1991
DOI: 10.2307/1163153
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Equations in Scientific Proofs: Effects on Comprehension

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
2
1
1
1

Citation Types

0
9
0

Year Published

1992
1992
2022
2022

Publication Types

Select...
5
1

Relationship

0
6

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 7 publications
(9 citation statements)
references
References 0 publications
0
9
0
Order By: Relevance
“…Yet, empirical findings about the relation between representational format, cognitive load, and learning outcomes are far from straightforward: the advantage of a particular format in learning is not universal, but depends on a complex interaction among the nature of the task and the material, the student's ability, prior knowledge, and practice time. For example, Dee-Lucas and Larkin (1991) found that learning with verbal material was superior to learning with equivalent equations. Leung et al (1997) replicated these findings but also found that this effect only applies to less able students.…”
Section: Representational Format and Cognitive Loadmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Yet, empirical findings about the relation between representational format, cognitive load, and learning outcomes are far from straightforward: the advantage of a particular format in learning is not universal, but depends on a complex interaction among the nature of the task and the material, the student's ability, prior knowledge, and practice time. For example, Dee-Lucas and Larkin (1991) found that learning with verbal material was superior to learning with equivalent equations. Leung et al (1997) replicated these findings but also found that this effect only applies to less able students.…”
Section: Representational Format and Cognitive Loadmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…When asked whether his knowledge of thermoconductivity would change his prediction, he said "no"; his prediction was consistent with molecular kinetic theory (Linn et al, in press). diSessa reports similar outcomes when physicists are asked to use scientific principles to explain the operation of vacuum cleaners and other common appliances (diSessa, 1983). If scientists cannot explain everyday phenomena in domains where they understand the great ideas, they are likely to be equally unsuccessful after taking a course introducing great ideas in other disciplines.…”
Section: Science Mattersmentioning
confidence: 91%
“…The heat-flow model and other pragmatic principles have proven helpful in guiding students' discovery and subsequent understanding of scientific phenomena. diSessa reports similar benefits from using computer microworlds to help students integrate their intuitive ideas about motion (diSessa, 1988). Fundamentally, pragmatic principles for middle school students provide abstractions that are macroscopic rather than microscopic and qualitative rather than quantitative.…”
Section: The Computer As Lab Partnermentioning
confidence: 95%
“…The studies [48], [49], which considered the content domains of Pascal's fluid pressure principle and simple interest calculation, have examined representations in the form of words, equations, or combinations thereof and compared the effectiveness of these representations with each other, but not with representations involving pictures/graphs. Note that if the assumptions of the outlined theory of cognition hold true, then these representations involving equations are processed in the verbal channel and do not utilize the resources of the pictorial channel.…”
Section: Related Workmentioning
confidence: 99%