1978
DOI: 10.3758/bf03336857
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Errors in a recognition memory task are a U-shaped function of word frequency

Abstract: College students rated verbal items for either familiarity or meaningfulness and were then tested for their recognition memory of rated items from four critical classes: nonwords and words appearing less than 1 time, 1-10, and more than 40 times per million words of written text (Thorndike-Lorge, 1944). Following either rating task, recognition errors were found to be a Ll-shaped function of word frequency and nonwords were recognized least well. Although a Ll-shaped function was expected on the basis of what … Show more

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
1
1
1

Citation Types

5
24
1

Year Published

1992
1992
2023
2023

Publication Types

Select...
7
1

Relationship

0
8

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 28 publications
(30 citation statements)
references
References 12 publications
5
24
1
Order By: Relevance
“…The same pattern of results was obtained with corrected hits (hits false alarms) as the measure of accuracy. Thus the present study replicates prior research with respect to item recognition; recognition accuracy was an inverted Ushaped function of word frequency, with highest accuracy for words of relatively low frequency and lower accuracy for words of high and very low frequency (e.g., Chalmers et al, 1997;Mandler et al, 1982;Rao & Proctor, 1984;Schulman, 1976;Wixted, 1992;Zechmeister, Curt, & Sebastian, 1978). 3 Figure 2 presents the results of the order reconstruction test.…”
Section: Resultssupporting
confidence: 59%
“…The same pattern of results was obtained with corrected hits (hits false alarms) as the measure of accuracy. Thus the present study replicates prior research with respect to item recognition; recognition accuracy was an inverted Ushaped function of word frequency, with highest accuracy for words of relatively low frequency and lower accuracy for words of high and very low frequency (e.g., Chalmers et al, 1997;Mandler et al, 1982;Rao & Proctor, 1984;Schulman, 1976;Wixted, 1992;Zechmeister, Curt, & Sebastian, 1978). 3 Figure 2 presents the results of the order reconstruction test.…”
Section: Resultssupporting
confidence: 59%
“…The user is presented with a cue that is an exact match for something that may be encoded, but not necessarily stored in memory (e.g. Shepard, 1967;Zechmeister et al, 1978). In contrast, recall is a measure of what has actually been passed into long term storage by the user and requires the user to search and retrieve information in storage with no extra cues provided (e.g.…”
Section: Article In Pressmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…The low-frequency advantage in recognition has since been confirmed in a number of studies over a range of conditions and manipulations (see Glanzer & Adams, 1985, for a review). When word frequency effects are studied over a larger range offrequencies (not just low vs. high), the relationship between frequency and accuracy is an inverted U-shaped function (Wixted, 1992;Zechmeister, Curt, & Sebastian, 1978). Recognition is highest for lowfrequency words and decreases as word frequency either increases or decreases.…”
mentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Nonwords are assumed to be poorly recognized either because they have no meaning or because they have never been encountered prior to the experiment. Although poor recognition of very low-frequency words has been linked to low ratings of lexicality (Schulman, 1976) and to low ratings of meaningfulness and familiarity (Zechmeister et al, 1978), the influence of these experiential factors has not been separated from other correlates ofword frequency (e.g., phonemic and graphemic composition). ' Allen and Garton (1968) adopted a naturalistic approach in their investigation of the word frequency effect.…”
mentioning
confidence: 99%