2018
DOI: 10.31234/osf.io/km28u
|View full text |Cite
Preprint
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Estimating Across-Trial Variability Parameters of the Diffusion Decision Model: Expert Advice and Recommendations

Abstract: For many years the Diffusion Decision Model (DDM) has successfully accounted for behavioral data from a wide range of domains. Important contributors to the DDM's success are the across-trial variability parameters, which allow the model to account for the various shapes of response time distributions encountered in practice. However, several researchers have pointed out that estimating the variability parameters can be a challenging task. Moreover, the numerous fitting methods for the DDM each come with their… Show more

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
1
1
1
1

Citation Types

0
17
0

Year Published

2019
2019
2022
2022

Publication Types

Select...
5
3

Relationship

4
4

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 12 publications
(17 citation statements)
references
References 38 publications
0
17
0
Order By: Relevance
“…However, the recent many-lab study of Dutilh et al (2018) where each research team had to decide which latent parameters varied across experimental conditions in 14 different experiments 1 -found reasonable discrepancies in conclusions between teams that used the diffusion model and teams that used the LBA, which were much larger than discrepancies between teams that used the same model. Furthermore, recent parameter recovery studies have provided a pessimistic perspective of the measurement properties of more complex EAMs, showing poor recovery for models with decision urgency that varies over the course of a decision , the LCA (Miletić, Turner, Forstmann, & van Maanen, 2017), and even the diffusion model when all random between-trial variability parameters are included (Lerche & Voss, 2016;Boehm et al, 2018). However, the inability to recover absolute parameter values does not necessarily mean that the relative differences between conditions in the values of specific parameters cannot be recovered, or that these more complex EAMs will differ from simpler EAMs in their conclusions about which latent parameters vary between experimental conditions and/or groups.…”
Section: A Standard Focus On Latent Parametersmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…However, the recent many-lab study of Dutilh et al (2018) where each research team had to decide which latent parameters varied across experimental conditions in 14 different experiments 1 -found reasonable discrepancies in conclusions between teams that used the diffusion model and teams that used the LBA, which were much larger than discrepancies between teams that used the same model. Furthermore, recent parameter recovery studies have provided a pessimistic perspective of the measurement properties of more complex EAMs, showing poor recovery for models with decision urgency that varies over the course of a decision , the LCA (Miletić, Turner, Forstmann, & van Maanen, 2017), and even the diffusion model when all random between-trial variability parameters are included (Lerche & Voss, 2016;Boehm et al, 2018). However, the inability to recover absolute parameter values does not necessarily mean that the relative differences between conditions in the values of specific parameters cannot be recovered, or that these more complex EAMs will differ from simpler EAMs in their conclusions about which latent parameters vary between experimental conditions and/or groups.…”
Section: A Standard Focus On Latent Parametersmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…This would explain why we found a slope near 1 between single-trial N200 peak-latencies and response times (see bottom right panel of Figure 6) but no evidence of this slope of 1 (albeit a significant and positive relationship) when fitting DDM parameters to N200 peak-latencies on single trials. Better theoretical models of behavior with trial-to-trial variability in NDT are probably needed (see Boehm et al, 2018), such as cognitive models that include both decision processes and mind-wandering processes (e.g. see van Vugt et al, 2015;Mittner et al, 2016).…”
Section: Evidence Against N200 Peak-latencies Tracking Visual Encodinmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Ratcliff & Rouder, 1998), and non-decision time (s ter ; Ratcliff & Tuerlinckx, 2002), the currently study only uses the "simple" (i.e., 4 parameter) diffusion model due to the poor measurement properties associated with the "full" (i.e., including all between-trial variabil-ity parameters) diffusion model (van Ravenzwaaij & Oberauer, 2009;Lerche & Voss, 2016;Lerche, Voss, & Nagler, 2017;Boehm et al, 2018;Evans, Tillman, & Wagenmakers, 2020).…”
Section: Formal Modelsmentioning
confidence: 99%