2018
DOI: 10.1177/2332858418756598
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Estimating the Consequences of Norway’s National Scale-Up of Early Childhood Education and Care (Beginning in Infancy) for Early Language Skills

Abstract: While most early childhood education and care (ECEC) programs taken to scale in the United States have served socially disadvantaged 3- to 5-years-olds, Norway scaled up universal ECEC from age 1. We investigated the consequences of Norway’s universal ECEC scale-up for children’s early language skills, exploiting variation in ECEC coverage across birth cohorts and municipalities in a population-based sample (n = 63,350). Estimates from two-stage least squares (i.e., instrumental variable) regression and genera… Show more

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
1
1
1
1

Citation Types

3
11
1
5

Year Published

2019
2019
2024
2024

Publication Types

Select...
7
1
1
1

Relationship

3
7

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 24 publications
(20 citation statements)
references
References 60 publications
3
11
1
5
Order By: Relevance
“…These efforts have focused on methods that do not rely entirely on measured variables to control for selection, such as instrumental variables, as well as fixed‐effects, difference‐in‐difference, and regression discontinuity analyses (e.g., Duncan et al, ; McCartney et al, ; Miller et al, , for further explanation of these issues). We agree with these ideas and have used these designs in our research (e.g., Dearing, Zachrisson, Mykletun, & Toppelberg, ; Dearing, Zachrisson, & Nærde, ; Zachrisson & Dearing, ). Yet many instances remain in which developmental researchers cannot use these methods, despite their strengths.…”
Section: Handling Selection Effects In Correlational Designssupporting
confidence: 68%
“…These efforts have focused on methods that do not rely entirely on measured variables to control for selection, such as instrumental variables, as well as fixed‐effects, difference‐in‐difference, and regression discontinuity analyses (e.g., Duncan et al, ; McCartney et al, ; Miller et al, , for further explanation of these issues). We agree with these ideas and have used these designs in our research (e.g., Dearing, Zachrisson, Mykletun, & Toppelberg, ; Dearing, Zachrisson, & Nærde, ; Zachrisson & Dearing, ). Yet many instances remain in which developmental researchers cannot use these methods, despite their strengths.…”
Section: Handling Selection Effects In Correlational Designssupporting
confidence: 68%
“…Disadvantaged children, however, may suffer from less developmental stimulation from parents, and their human capital accumulation is more dependent on the learning environment in kindergarten and school. Such differential sensitivity to instructional quality by socioeconomic background is consistent with evidence that school and preschool programs are particularly important for disadvantaged children (Borman & Kimball, 2005;Dearing, Zachrisson, Mykletun, & Toppelberg, 2018;Havnes & 4 There is some evidence, however, suggesting a positive interaction effect of family and school resources. Kim (2001) finds that low-educated parents cut back on their childcare time use in response to an increase in per pupil school expenditure.…”
Section: Socioeconomic Gradientssupporting
confidence: 67%
“…Norway offers universal subsidized ECEC to families beginning when their children are 1 year of age, and prior to this families have paid parental leave. Use of informal care is rare (Dearing, Zachrisson, Mykletun, & Toppelberg, 2018). Children almost exclusively attend full day care, though the amount of hours children actually spend in care varies, averaging 34 hours per week at age 2 in the sample used in the present study (Dearing et al, 2015), a weekly average reflecting that some parents either work part time or flexi-time, thus reducing their children's time in ECEC.…”
Section: A Quasi-experimental Test Of Age Of Entry Effects On Parenting Quality In Norwaymentioning
confidence: 95%