2019
DOI: 10.1108/ijcma-03-2018-0044
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Examination of knowledge hiding with conflict, competition and personal values

Abstract: Purpose The purpose of this paper was to examine knowledge hiding behaviours with perceived conflict types, competition and personal values of employees. Design/methodology/approach Two studies were carried out and structural equation modelling and moderated regression analyses were conducted to test the hypotheses. Findings Study I, with employees from software development companies, revealed that task conflict and relationship conflict have additive effect on knowledge hiding behaviour. Additionally, tas… Show more

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
1
1
1
1

Citation Types

8
106
0

Year Published

2020
2020
2021
2021

Publication Types

Select...
9

Relationship

0
9

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 99 publications
(114 citation statements)
references
References 80 publications
8
106
0
Order By: Relevance
“…Last but not least, we want to emphasize that our day-level results did not completely generalize to the person level, supporting the notion that day-level relationships do not simply mirror person-level relationships (see Gabriel et al, 2019;McCormick et al, 2020) and pointing to the usefulness of employing a diary approach to study knowledge hiding, its antecedents, and consequences. Specifically, while relationship conflict was positively related to playing dumb and evasive hiding also at the person level-replicating earlier findings (Semerci, 2019) on the predictive role of interpersonal conflict for knowledge hiding-playing dumb and evasive hiding were not significantly related to psychological strain responses at the between-person level. Yet, for evasive hiding the relationships even showed a positive trend.…”
Section: Theoretical and Empirical Implicationssupporting
confidence: 81%
See 1 more Smart Citation
“…Last but not least, we want to emphasize that our day-level results did not completely generalize to the person level, supporting the notion that day-level relationships do not simply mirror person-level relationships (see Gabriel et al, 2019;McCormick et al, 2020) and pointing to the usefulness of employing a diary approach to study knowledge hiding, its antecedents, and consequences. Specifically, while relationship conflict was positively related to playing dumb and evasive hiding also at the person level-replicating earlier findings (Semerci, 2019) on the predictive role of interpersonal conflict for knowledge hiding-playing dumb and evasive hiding were not significantly related to psychological strain responses at the between-person level. Yet, for evasive hiding the relationships even showed a positive trend.…”
Section: Theoretical and Empirical Implicationssupporting
confidence: 81%
“…In our study, though, we captured interpersonal conflict with one or more coworkers as well as knowledge hiding behaviors toward one or more coworkers. While recent studies on the social predictors of knowledge hiding used the same approach (e.g., Jahanzeb et al, 2019;Semerci, 2019), this way of how we measured conflict and knowledge hiding does not guarantee that conflict partners and knowledge hiding targets were the same persons. However, the social interactionist framework (Andersson and Pearson, 1999) and empirical findings indicate that employees do not only engage in direct retaliation toward conflict partners, but that experienced conflict might incite them to engage in a rather broad set of negative interpersonal behaviors toward a range of targets (e.g., Penney and Spector, 2005;Liu et al, 2015).…”
Section: Limitations and Directions For Researchmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Distrust similarly led to knowledge hiding among 345 Canadian finance professionals, students, and an online panel (Connelly et al, 2012) and among 19 R&D professionals in a pharmaceutical company in India (Kumar Jha & Varkkey, 2018). Other negative experiences such as coworker opportunistic behaviors also increased knowledge hoarding (Anaza & Nowlin, 2017), as did the experience of negative interpersonal conflicts, which were studied among 560 respondents from software development and banking in Turkey (Boz Semerci, 2019). As another example, Serenko and Bontis (2016) collected data from 693 employees of the credit union in Canada and the United States on reciprocity “loops.” The authors find that a lack of reciprocity of one person could lead to knowledge hiding of another who, in turn, negatively reciprocates by also hiding.…”
Section: Empirical Review and Integrative Frameworkmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…In line with previous research on knowledge-hiding behavior(Semerci, 2019) and recommendations about structural equation modeling(Lattin, Carroll, & Green, 2003), we model this behavior as a first-order multi-item construct for the comparison of different structural equation models, as reported subsequently in Table3.…”
mentioning
confidence: 99%