2002
DOI: 10.1016/s0388-0001(01)00036-5
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Exaptation and English stress

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
4
1

Citation Types

1
4
0

Year Published

2003
2003
2022
2022

Publication Types

Select...
4
3
1

Relationship

0
8

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 15 publications
(5 citation statements)
references
References 7 publications
1
4
0
Order By: Relevance
“…Finally, the noun-verb stress contrast also seems to be cognitively functional as it facilitates online processing, particularly in individuals with limited vocabularies (Kawamoto, Farrar & Overbeek, 1993, cited in Sereno & Jongman, 1995; Arciuli & Cupples, 2003, 2006). These findings lend empirical support to McCully's (2002: 343) assessment that “[s]tress marking is being used signally […]”.…”
Section: Introductionsupporting
confidence: 67%
See 2 more Smart Citations
“…Finally, the noun-verb stress contrast also seems to be cognitively functional as it facilitates online processing, particularly in individuals with limited vocabularies (Kawamoto, Farrar & Overbeek, 1993, cited in Sereno & Jongman, 1995; Arciuli & Cupples, 2003, 2006). These findings lend empirical support to McCully's (2002: 343) assessment that “[s]tress marking is being used signally […]”.…”
Section: Introductionsupporting
confidence: 67%
“…Nouns display a strong tendency to be stressed on syllables at, or closer to, the beginnings of words (‘left-hand stress’), while verbs tend to be stressed word-finally (‘right-hand stress’). This paper investigates the potential of the stress contrast as an indicator of word class (McCully, 2002). Specifically, it pursues the question whether class-specific stress can assist language learners in their acquisition of nouns and verbs, respectively, by acting as a phonological cue for their categorisation and storage in the mental lexicon (henceforth ‘stress typicality hypothesis’).…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 99%
See 1 more Smart Citation
“…Examining stress patterns up to and including Chaucer, but not beyond, Minkova (1997) rejected earlier proposals that either the OF/AN, or the Latin stress rule had displaced the left-edge Germanic stress in pre-1400 English; similarly, Redford (2003), adding a hypothesis of ‘hovering’ stress for doublets in verse 31 . McCully (2002) looks at post-1500 OED data, and McCully (2003: 355) talks of ‘the phonological crisis provided by the spectacle of incoming romance rules’, and provides arguments against what he labels ‘the catastrophe model’; his focus is on evaluating the empirical validity of competing descriptive models, but as far as source ME data go, it is still Chaucer/Chaucerian versus later, or contemporary English, as in the most recent study of historical noun–verb stress contrasts (Hofmann 2020). Should one need further justification for our diachronic fact-finding mission, one particular recurrent statement about the chronology of prosodic change in English attempts a more explicit timeline for the stress innovations in PDE: Sequence of changes in stress parameters: 1400 : Foot direction Leftward, Main stress Left (as in OE) 1530 : Foot direction Rightward , Main stress Left 1660: Foot direction Rightward, Main stress Right (Cited from Fikkert, Dresher & Lahiri (2006: 146); also in Dresher & Lahiri (2005: 82–3), Dresher (2013: 61), Lahiri (2015: 231), Dresher & Lahiri (2015) 32 …”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…English is a language in which lexical stress can be movable (e.g., PERmit is a strong-weak sequence associated with a noun, whereas perMIT is a weak-strong sequence associated with a verb; McCully, 2002). However, trochaic (i.e., strong-weak) nouns are far more frequent than iambic (i.e., weak-strong) nouns.…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 99%