2016
DOI: 10.1016/j.dr.2016.06.005
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Executive function in the first three years of life: Precursors, predictors and patterns

Abstract: Executive function (EF) underpins the ability to set goals and work towards those goals by co-ordinating thought and action. Its emergence during the first 3 years of life is understudied, largely due to the limitations that early social, motor and language skills place on performance on traditional EF tasks. Nevertheless, across the fields of cognitive psychology, neuroscience, social development and temperament research, evidence is amassing of meaningful precursors and predictors of EF. This review draws to… Show more

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
2
1
1
1

Citation Types

13
200
4

Year Published

2017
2017
2024
2024

Publication Types

Select...
8
1

Relationship

0
9

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 202 publications
(217 citation statements)
references
References 238 publications
(369 reference statements)
13
200
4
Order By: Relevance
“…Rather, given the stability in function between ages 5.5 and 18 years they imply that any catch-up would occur before the age of 5. This is also in line with findings regarding typical executive function development during the first 5 years of life, in normative groups of children (Diamond, 2013;Garon et al, 2008;Hendry, Jones, & Charman, 2016). An implication of our results is that a wait-and-see approach will more likely lead to secondary effects of a deficit rather than a catch-up over time.…”
Section: Discussionsupporting
confidence: 77%
“…Rather, given the stability in function between ages 5.5 and 18 years they imply that any catch-up would occur before the age of 5. This is also in line with findings regarding typical executive function development during the first 5 years of life, in normative groups of children (Diamond, 2013;Garon et al, 2008;Hendry, Jones, & Charman, 2016). An implication of our results is that a wait-and-see approach will more likely lead to secondary effects of a deficit rather than a catch-up over time.…”
Section: Discussionsupporting
confidence: 77%
“…A similar idea has recently been put forward by several researchers in the field, but their hypothesis relates primarily to attention in late infancy in relation to toddler and early childhood EFs -there is good evidence for such a link (Garon et al, 2008;Hendry et al, 2016;Johansson et al, 2015). A similar idea has recently been put forward by several researchers in the field, but their hypothesis relates primarily to attention in late infancy in relation to toddler and early childhood EFs -there is good evidence for such a link (Garon et al, 2008;Hendry et al, 2016;Johansson et al, 2015).…”
Section: Research Highlightsmentioning
confidence: 69%
“…More recently, there has been a renewed interest in the development of EFs and their precursors during infancy and toddlerhood (Hendry, Jones, & Charman, 2016;Johansson, Marciszko, Gredebäck, Nyström, & Bohlin, 2015). Once verbal instruction is possible (usually around 2-2.5 years of age), children can be asked to inhibit actions, maintain and manipulate information in WM, and switch between different tasks (e.g., Garon, Smith, & Bryson, 2014;Mulder, Hoofs, Verhagen, van der Veen, & Leseman, 2014;Zelazo, Frye, & Rapus, 1996).…”
Section: A Wealth Of Knowledge Already Exists On the Development Ofmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Both cases require that students play attention and inhibit other stimuli to make the necessary selfcorrections. Other studies also show that students with learning disorders have impaired monitoring skills [33][34][35] .…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 99%