2015
DOI: 10.1016/j.concog.2015.02.015
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Exogenous attention to unseen objects?

Abstract: The full-text may be used and/or reproduced, and given to third parties in any format or medium, without prior permission or charge, for personal research or study, educational, or not-for-pro t purposes provided that:• a full bibliographic reference is made to the original source • a link is made to the metadata record in DRO • the full-text is not changed in any way The full-text must not be sold in any format or medium without the formal permission of the copyright holders.Please consult the full DRO policy… Show more

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
2
1
1
1

Citation Types

0
8
0

Year Published

2015
2015
2020
2020

Publication Types

Select...
5

Relationship

0
5

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 9 publications
(8 citation statements)
references
References 47 publications
0
8
0
Order By: Relevance
“…However, other researchers have argued that we are conscious of much more information we can cognitively access and report on (Lamme 2010; Block 2011). Research furthermore indicates that visual consciousness and attention can be (neurally) dissociated (Koch and Tsuchiya 2007; Wyart and Tallon-Baudry 2008; Norman et al 2015). Notably, not only AB studies (Slagter et al 2010; Slagter et al 2012), but also studies using simple backward masking tasks with only one stimulus (Christensen et al 2006; Van Opstal et al 2014; Bisenius et al 2015), in which conscious access is not dependent on attentional selection as in the AB task (Dehaene and Changeux 2011), have reported greater activity in the striatum to consciously perceived stimuli.…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…However, other researchers have argued that we are conscious of much more information we can cognitively access and report on (Lamme 2010; Block 2011). Research furthermore indicates that visual consciousness and attention can be (neurally) dissociated (Koch and Tsuchiya 2007; Wyart and Tallon-Baudry 2008; Norman et al 2015). Notably, not only AB studies (Slagter et al 2010; Slagter et al 2012), but also studies using simple backward masking tasks with only one stimulus (Christensen et al 2006; Van Opstal et al 2014; Bisenius et al 2015), in which conscious access is not dependent on attentional selection as in the AB task (Dehaene and Changeux 2011), have reported greater activity in the striatum to consciously perceived stimuli.…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Curiously, a mixed strategy has also been used in which the direct measure is tested using B, whereas a p-value is still used to test the indirect measure (e.g., Norman et al, 2015). This mixed method is probably used because the main advantage of B over the p-value is when H 0 is the hypothesis of interest.…”
Section: Statistical Analysis Of Subliminal Primingmentioning
confidence: 98%
“…Furthermore, experiments vary in their sample size (N) and number of trials (n) used. Therefore, we decided to vary these parameters based on previous studies (e.g., González-García et al, 2015;Huang et al, 2014;Jusyte & Schönenberg, 2014;Kido & Makioka, 2015;Kiefer et al, 2015;Lin & Murray, 2015;Marcos Malmierca, 2015;Norman et al, 2015;Ocampo, 2015;Ocampo et al, 2015;Schoeberl et al, 2015;Wildegger et al, 2015) systematically in two simulations.…”
Section: Simulations 3a and 3b: Varying Parametersmentioning
confidence: 99%
See 1 more Smart Citation
“…Using the classic object-based attention paradigm developed by Egly, Driver, and Rafal (1994), Norman, Heywood and Kentridge (2015) extend our knowledge of deployment of attention by unconsciously processed stimuli by showing that significant effects of object-based attention can be obtained for visual objects that are processed at unconscious levels.…”
Section: Unconscious Deployment/allocation Of Space-based and Object-mentioning
confidence: 98%