2015
DOI: 10.1037/a0039036
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Expanding the scope of memory search: Modeling intralist and interlist effects in free recall.

Abstract: The human memory system is remarkable in its capacity to focus its search on items learned in a given context. This capacity can be so precise that many leading models of human memory assume that only those items learned in the context of a recently studied list compete for recall. We sought to extend the explanatory scope of these models to include not only intralist phenomena, such as primacy and recency effects, but also interlist phenomena such as proactive and retroactive interference. Building on retriev… Show more

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
2
1
1
1

Citation Types

10
314
0

Year Published

2016
2016
2024
2024

Publication Types

Select...
6
2

Relationship

3
5

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 187 publications
(362 citation statements)
references
References 124 publications
(297 reference statements)
10
314
0
Order By: Relevance
“…For example, retrieved context models (e.g., Lohnas, Polyn, & Kahana, submitted; Polyn et al, 2009) assume primacy is due to increased attention to early list items whereas recency is due to mental context states at retrieval providing strong cues for recently presented items. We return to this point in the discussion.…”
Section: Resultsmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…For example, retrieved context models (e.g., Lohnas, Polyn, & Kahana, submitted; Polyn et al, 2009) assume primacy is due to increased attention to early list items whereas recency is due to mental context states at retrieval providing strong cues for recently presented items. We return to this point in the discussion.…”
Section: Resultsmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…To a large extent this problem was solved by developing formal models of benchmark effects in a small number of laboratory paradigms. This modeling work has led to a broad consensus about many fundamental aspects of memory including the importance of context–based cues in retrieval (e.g., Bower, 1967; Davelaar et al, 2005; Estes, 1955; Farrell, 2012; Howard & Kahana, 2002a; Lohnas, Polyn, & Kahana, 2015; Mensink & Raaijmakers, 1988; Murdock, 1997; Polyn et al, 2009; Sederberg, Howard, & Kahana, 2008). We can leverage this existing computational framework to help make the complexity of the free recall aging pattern tractable.…”
Section: Our Approach To Theory Buildingmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Therefore, we take the approach of implementing each of the theories within the retrieved context framework. The retrieved context framework has been extensively used to model the free recall effects outlined above in young adult samples (Howard & Kahana, 2002a; Lohnas et al, 2015; Polyn et al, 2009; Sederberg et al, 2008). The retrieved context framework provides a good test bed for at least two reasons.…”
Section: Our Approach To Theory Buildingmentioning
confidence: 99%
See 1 more Smart Citation
“…Instead, participants are asked whether they've seen the item in a particular episode, namely the study list. Thus, in a majority of episodic memory models, there is a context representation that defines the study list episode; the extent to which this context representation is different from prior contexts determines the extent to which participants are protected from interference from episodes prior to the study list (Dennis & Humphreys, 2001;Klein, Shiffrin, & Criss, 2007;Lohnas, Polyn, & Kahana, 2015). Although episodic memory models are often agnostic as to what defines context, a candidate explanation is the participant's cognitive and/or emotional state along with aspects of their physical surroundings.…”
Section: A Global Matching Model Of Source Memorymentioning
confidence: 99%