2014
DOI: 10.1111/ibi.12140
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Experimental evidence for parental, but not parentally biased, favouritism in relation to offspring size inBlueTitsCyanistes caeruleus

Abstract: In species with biparental care, males and females share the benefits of investing in offspring but pay the costs individually. As a result of these evolutionary conflicts of interest between the sexes, it is expected that the two parents should follow different behavioural rules when providing food to the young. Such a discrepancy may be accentuated when parents have to choose between different subsets of offspring (e.g. large and small nestlings). We manipulated the degree of hatching asynchrony in Blue Tits… Show more

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
2
1
1
1

Citation Types

1
8
0

Year Published

2015
2015
2023
2023

Publication Types

Select...
5
1
1

Relationship

1
6

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 15 publications
(9 citation statements)
references
References 58 publications
1
8
0
Order By: Relevance
“…We also found that males generally brought larger prey than females, which is the same sex-bias documented in some other passerines such as Pied Flycatchers (Ficedula hypoleuca) and Blue Tits (Bańbura et al 2001, Wiebe andSlagsvold 2014). Females are probably more attuned to the needs of their nestlings (Budden and Beissinger 2009, García-Navas et al 2014, Liu et al 2014 and may therefore bring smaller prey suitable for young nestlings to ensure they get sufficient food. Alternatively, females may not be intentionally searching for small prey but only bring it because they may be spending more time looking after the nestlings in the box, i.e., brooding, than males and may have less time to find larger prey (Wiebe and Slagsvold 2009).…”
Section: Prey Volumesupporting
confidence: 82%
“…We also found that males generally brought larger prey than females, which is the same sex-bias documented in some other passerines such as Pied Flycatchers (Ficedula hypoleuca) and Blue Tits (Bańbura et al 2001, Wiebe andSlagsvold 2014). Females are probably more attuned to the needs of their nestlings (Budden and Beissinger 2009, García-Navas et al 2014, Liu et al 2014 and may therefore bring smaller prey suitable for young nestlings to ensure they get sufficient food. Alternatively, females may not be intentionally searching for small prey but only bring it because they may be spending more time looking after the nestlings in the box, i.e., brooding, than males and may have less time to find larger prey (Wiebe and Slagsvold 2009).…”
Section: Prey Volumesupporting
confidence: 82%
“…Furthermore, great tit chicks showed larger fledging rates, probably because they hatched earlier and were larger (Slagsvold 1998). Larger chick size is advantageous because tit parents prioritise the feeding of larger chicks during early development stages (Dickens et al 2008;García-Navas et al 2014b). Thus, when food is a limiting factor, smaller chicks (i.e.…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…We expected that (i) the prevalence of facultative IBP would be greater in plots with higher nest-box occupation rates (Yom-Tov 1980, because nest-hole shortage could prevent all breeding birds from having their own nests, once food availability is not a limiting factor in plots of smaller size (Nour et al 1998). As the food provisioning by tit parents is mediated by chick size, with larger chicks receiving more supplies, at least in the early development stages (Dickens et al 2008;García-Navas et al 2014b), we expected that (ii) fledging rate of blue tits in mixed-species broods would be lower than that of their great tit broodmates. Additionally, (iii) it would be more advantageous for great tits to dump into blue tit nests than for blue tits to dump into great tit nests, because the larger size of great tit chicks makes them better competitors for parents' food (Slagsvold 1998).…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Thus, parents could potentially rely on both carotenoid-chroma and total brightness to discriminate offspring sex while adjusting their feeding strategies. Indeed, in other study populations, blue tit males and females receive different food items (García-Navas et al, 2014) or the total amount of investment (Dickens & Hartley, 2007). In addition, fledging yellow plumage could play a signaling role in family flocks that are formed immediately after fledging (Stenning, 2018) and during social interactions within flocks (Tschirren et al, 2005).…”
Section: Differences Between the Sexesmentioning
confidence: 99%