2010
DOI: 10.1016/j.jaa.2009.10.002
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Explaining prehistoric variation in the abundance of large prey: A zooarchaeological analysis of deer and rabbit hunting along the Pecho Coast of Central California

Abstract: Three main hypotheses are commonly employed to explain diachronic variation in the relative abun dance of remains of large terrestrial herbivores: (1) large prey populations decline as a function of anthro pogenic overexploitation; (2) large prey tends to increase as a result of increasing social payoffs; and (3) proportions of large terrestrial prey are dependent on stochastic fluctuations in climate. This paper tests predictions derived from these three hypotheses through a zooarchaeological analysis of elev… Show more

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
1
1
1
1

Citation Types

0
13
0

Year Published

2010
2010
2019
2019

Publication Types

Select...
5
3
1

Relationship

1
8

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 48 publications
(13 citation statements)
references
References 121 publications
(166 reference statements)
0
13
0
Order By: Relevance
“…Here we suggest that this is a function of stacking pursuit success probabilities resulting from sequential encounters on a single foraging bout (see also Bliege Bird et al, 2009;Codding et al, 2010). While pursuing large, highly mobile prey has a higher probability in ending in failure than pursuing smaller, less mobile prey, this probability of failure is a product of low post-encounter pursuit success ; see also Hawkes et al, 1991;Jochim, 1976;Lee, 1968Lee, , 1979.…”
Section: Traditional and Alternative Interpretations: Efficiency And mentioning
confidence: 74%
See 1 more Smart Citation
“…Here we suggest that this is a function of stacking pursuit success probabilities resulting from sequential encounters on a single foraging bout (see also Bliege Bird et al, 2009;Codding et al, 2010). While pursuing large, highly mobile prey has a higher probability in ending in failure than pursuing smaller, less mobile prey, this probability of failure is a product of low post-encounter pursuit success ; see also Hawkes et al, 1991;Jochim, 1976;Lee, 1968Lee, , 1979.…”
Section: Traditional and Alternative Interpretations: Efficiency And mentioning
confidence: 74%
“…Similar to R 2 values in OLS, R 2 L values can be interpreted as a reduction in unexplained deviance by the inclusion of the independent variables, thus models with higher values reduce more unexplained deviance than models with lower values. R 2 L values can be calculated easily from the GLM output of most statistical programs (see Menard, 2000Menard, , 2002; see also Codding et al, 2010).…”
Section: Methodsmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…“Adaptation” may be an unfashionable concept, but everyone knows that people in the past did something of the sort—especially hunter‐gatherers. Variations on behavioral ecology continue to work to explain prey choice, from deer and rabbit exploitation in California (Codding et al 2010) and oysters and fish in Korea (Kim 2010) to extreme salmon specialization in British Columbia (Coupland et al 2010). (Let us note, while talking of meat, that the exploitation of plant foods did not wait for the Holocene.…”
Section: Environment and Subsistence: How Much Agency?mentioning
confidence: 99%
“…The predictions of these models have been born out time and time again in the zooarchaeological record of California (though see Codding et al, 2010;Whitaker, 2008Whitaker, , 2009a.…”
Section: Resource Depression In Californiamentioning
confidence: 99%