2009
DOI: 10.1016/j.brat.2009.02.012
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Explication of interspousal criticality bias

Abstract: Although bias towards perceiving spousal criticism is related to dysphoria and marital discord (Smith & Peterson, 2008), the bias construct has received insufficient elaboration. We explicated the criticality bias construct by exploring its correlates and incremental validity relative to perceived criticism, marital attributions, and negative affect. 118 couples completed self-report measures and undertook a videotaped discussion task. Signal detection analyses of both spouses' and outside observers' ratings o… Show more

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
1
1
1

Citation Types

1
25
0

Year Published

2010
2010
2020
2020

Publication Types

Select...
6
1
1

Relationship

2
6

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 18 publications
(26 citation statements)
references
References 50 publications
1
25
0
Order By: Relevance
“…For example, people in unhappy marriages often attribute responsibility for negative behaviors to their partner (e.g., “Don came home late because he doesn’t care about his family”), while not attributing responsibility for positive behaviors to the partner (e.g., “Don came home early because his boss told him to do so”) (Bradbury, Beach, Fincham, & Nelson, 1996; Durtschi, Fincham, Cui, Lorenz, & Conger, 2011). Similarly, a “criticality bias” to misattribute a partner’s verbal and nonverbal communication as criticism (Smith & Peterson, 2008) is also associated with expressing criticism towards partners, using a negative tone in conversations, and greater “demanding” behaviors (Peterson, Smith, & Windle, 2009). While the specific role of social-cognitive processes in physical health remains understudied, attributing responsibility to the partner for negative behaviors predicted slower cortisol recovery following a conflict discussion in dating couples (Laurent & Powers, 2006).…”
Section: Explanatory Theoriesmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…For example, people in unhappy marriages often attribute responsibility for negative behaviors to their partner (e.g., “Don came home late because he doesn’t care about his family”), while not attributing responsibility for positive behaviors to the partner (e.g., “Don came home early because his boss told him to do so”) (Bradbury, Beach, Fincham, & Nelson, 1996; Durtschi, Fincham, Cui, Lorenz, & Conger, 2011). Similarly, a “criticality bias” to misattribute a partner’s verbal and nonverbal communication as criticism (Smith & Peterson, 2008) is also associated with expressing criticism towards partners, using a negative tone in conversations, and greater “demanding” behaviors (Peterson, Smith, & Windle, 2009). While the specific role of social-cognitive processes in physical health remains understudied, attributing responsibility to the partner for negative behaviors predicted slower cortisol recovery following a conflict discussion in dating couples (Laurent & Powers, 2006).…”
Section: Explanatory Theoriesmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…The patient who tends to draw more negative attributions about the relative’s behavior or events in the relative’s life (e.g., that the relative intended to be hurtful or that the event stems from a negative and stable character trait of the relative) may be more likely to interpret the relative’s behavior toward himself or herself as destructive criticism. Indeed, Peterson, Smith, and Windle (2009) found a self-report questionnaire assessing spouses’ attributions for their partners’ negative behaviors in general was correlated to criticality bias scores obtained from a subsequent laboratory interaction between spouses. Moreover, perceived criticism is significantly and negatively correlated with marital adjustment (Chambless & Blake, 2009; Smith & Peterson, 2008) and marital adjustment is predicted both concurrently and longitudinally by negative attributions (Bradbury & Fincham, 1990; Fincham, Bradbury, Arias, Byrne, & Karney, 1997).…”
mentioning
confidence: 99%
“…We hypothesized that participants who more strongly (Study 1) or more frequently (Study 2) endorsed negative attributions about their relatives’ behavior during a problem-solving interaction would be more likely to perceive their relatives as critical of them than would those who made fewer negative attributions or endorsed a weaker belief in these attributions. In addition, drawing from Peterson et al (2009), we expected attributions would uniquely contribute to the prediction of perceived criticism above and beyond the contribution of criticism as rated by observers.…”
mentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Study procedures have previously been described (Peterson, Smith, & Windle, ). Both members of the couple independently completed self‐report measures, a structured clinical interview, and an audiotaped FMSS that prompted them to independently talk about “what kind of person [spouse's name] is, and how the two of you get along.” Participants then jointly completed a marital interaction task, for which one person was selected to be the “focus” of the conversation, that is, the person to whom the conversation topic pertained.…”
Section: Methodsmentioning
confidence: 99%