2006
DOI: 10.1080/13506280600574487
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Explicit memory for rejected distractors during visual search

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
2
1
1

Citation Types

7
34
2

Year Published

2006
2006
2021
2021

Publication Types

Select...
8

Relationship

1
7

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 33 publications
(43 citation statements)
references
References 45 publications
7
34
2
Order By: Relevance
“…In addition, at least in a long-term visual memory task, the timing of the target presentation does not dramatically influence the memory obtained. This result contrasts with previously reported recency effects in visual memory in both visual search (Beck et al, 2006) and scene viewing (Hollingworth, 2004;Tatler et al, 2005). However, as was described in the discussion of the first experiment, it is not surprising that testing memory after the presentation of hundreds of objects eliminates the recency advantage observed in those studies.…”
Section: Resultscontrasting
confidence: 81%
See 2 more Smart Citations
“…In addition, at least in a long-term visual memory task, the timing of the target presentation does not dramatically influence the memory obtained. This result contrasts with previously reported recency effects in visual memory in both visual search (Beck et al, 2006) and scene viewing (Hollingworth, 2004;Tatler et al, 2005). However, as was described in the discussion of the first experiment, it is not surprising that testing memory after the presentation of hundreds of objects eliminates the recency advantage observed in those studies.…”
Section: Resultscontrasting
confidence: 81%
“…This type of memory test eliminates the ability of the participant to rely on the semantic knowledge that a black cat had been presented to successfully perform the selection; the participant must rely on his or her memory for the visual details of the objects. Interestingly, the longterm memory performance for the target objects in Williams et al was TARGET AND DISTRACTOR VISUAL MEMORY 203 similar to the immediate memory test performance of participants in previous studies of visual short-term memory in visual search and scene memorization (e.g., Beck, Peterson, Boot, Vomela, & Kramer, 2006;Hollingworth, 2004Hollingworth, , 2005, indicating that although the memory test was considerably delayed in Williams et al, visual memory for these objects was robust. However, memory for distractor objects in Williams et al was Figure 1.…”
Section: Williamssupporting
confidence: 56%
See 1 more Smart Citation
“…The percentage of correct object identifications in a test phase depended on the number of intervening fixations and resulted in a recency effect that was very similar to ours. A similar approach was used by Beck, Peterson, Boot, Vomela, and Kramer (2006) to study explicit object memory in search. On 33% of the trials, they ended a trial prematurely and asked participants to identify which of two items had been presented at a Number of Fixations…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…In this case, refixations were therefore used to acquire a specific piece of information, the spatial information needed to correctly position a block in the model reconstruction. These fundamentally different functions of refixations complicate even the simplest of generalizations across tasks; whereas accuracy in a scene memory task improves with the number of refixations during study (Holm & Mäntyla, 2007), refixations in a search task are generally associated with poor spatial memory and inefficient processing (Beck, Peterson, Boot, Vomela, & Kramer, 2006;Dickinson & Zelinsky, 2005;Gilchrist & Harvey, 2000).…”
mentioning
confidence: 99%