2014
DOI: 10.1002/bsl.2144
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Exploring Parent–Child Discussions of Crime and their Influence on Children's Memory

Abstract: When children witness or experience criminal events, the first people they go to are generally parents. Typically, no one else is privy to these conversations, and consequently little is known about their specific content. Research has shown that children can be quite accurate witnesses at times. However, they can also incorporate information from misleading and suggestive questions into their recall, and once their event memory has been changed, children may be unable to provide accurate reports. It is import… Show more

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
1
1
1
1

Citation Types

1
5
0

Year Published

2016
2016
2022
2022

Publication Types

Select...
6

Relationship

0
6

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 12 publications
(6 citation statements)
references
References 25 publications
1
5
0
Order By: Relevance
“…First, we were interested in how biasing information influenced parental conversation style. Consistent with previous research (Goodman et al 1995;Principe et al 2013;Warren and Peterson 2014), it was predicted that biased parents would provide more misinformation during the parentchild conversations than parents in the other experimental conditions. It was also expected that parents in the biased discussion condition, due to a reliance on mostly self-produced misinformation, would engage in a more repetitive, close-ended discussion style than parents in the non-biased discussion condition (who would need to rely on i child-generated information).…”
Section: The Present Studysupporting
confidence: 74%
See 2 more Smart Citations
“…First, we were interested in how biasing information influenced parental conversation style. Consistent with previous research (Goodman et al 1995;Principe et al 2013;Warren and Peterson 2014), it was predicted that biased parents would provide more misinformation during the parentchild conversations than parents in the other experimental conditions. It was also expected that parents in the biased discussion condition, due to a reliance on mostly self-produced misinformation, would engage in a more repetitive, close-ended discussion style than parents in the non-biased discussion condition (who would need to rely on i child-generated information).…”
Section: The Present Studysupporting
confidence: 74%
“…However, in direct contrast to the findings from Goodman et al (1995), these findings suggest that parental biases negatively affect young children's eyewitness memory reports. Consistent with Principe et al's (2013) findings, Warren and Peterson (2014) examined conversations between 7-to 10-year-old children and their parents about a crime video. Although Warren and Peterson did not directly manipulate bias, they did find that children incorporated about 40% of the information that parents introduced through misleading and suggestive questions.…”
Section: Attachment and Parental Reminiscingmentioning
confidence: 67%
See 1 more Smart Citation
“…However, questions of these very same forms could have dire consequences in the forensic arena if the information suggested by parents is inaccurate and children later report it as fact. Indeed, studies examining how parents question their children about simulated crimes demonstrate that parents naturally rely on these sorts of probes to elicit recall (Warren & Peterson, 2014), suggesting that even when parents are working to extract the truth, they may use what would be construed as biased questions in the forensic interviewing arena. Thus, given that parents use the kinds of questions that can elicit false reports in both everyday and forensic recall settings, understanding the conditions that can prompt parents to do so and the resulting influences on children's accuracy is a critical issue for the study of children's eyewitness testimony.…”
Section: Everyday Versus Forensic Memory Sharingmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Our search did not reveal any new studies examining socioeconomic status (SES) and suggestibility, but it did uncover eight studies examining gender differences in suggestibility (Eisen, Goodman, Qin, Davis, & Crayton, 2007;Ghetti, Papini, & Angelini, 2006;Gilstrap & Ceci, 2005;Gudjonsson, Vagni, Maiorano, & Pajardi, 2016;Kim, Kwon, & Ceci, 2017;Uhl, Camilletti, Scullin, & Wood, 2016;Volpini, Melis, Petralia, & Rosenberg, 2016;Warren & Peterson, 2014). Only one found a significant association between gender and suggestibility, such that boys were more likely to acquiesce to misleading questions (Gilstrap & Ceci, 2005).…”
Section: Demographic Factorsmentioning
confidence: 94%