2012
DOI: 10.1371/journal.pone.0051417
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Eye-Hand Synergy and Intermittent Behaviors during Target-Directed Tracking with Visual and Non-visual Information

Abstract: Visual feedback and non-visual information play different roles in tracking of an external target. This study explored the respective roles of the visual and non-visual information in eleven healthy volunteers who coupled the manual cursor to a rhythmically moving target of 0.5 Hz under three sensorimotor conditions: eye-alone tracking (EA), eye-hand tracking with visual feedback of manual outputs (EH tracking), and the same tracking without such feedback (EHM tracking). Tracking error, kinematic variables, an… Show more

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
3
2

Citation Types

3
16
1

Year Published

2013
2013
2021
2021

Publication Types

Select...
7

Relationship

1
6

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 19 publications
(20 citation statements)
references
References 60 publications
3
16
1
Order By: Relevance
“…Changes in tracking gain, temporal lag or tracking error between target and pointer trajectories are measurable evidence of these improvements (Huang and Hwang, 2012). Visuo-manual coordination in tracking tasks is often considered to be controlled by a combination of feed-forward and feedback adaptive internal models (Neilson et al, 1988;Miall et al, 1993;Shams and Seitz, 2008).…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 99%
See 1 more Smart Citation
“…Changes in tracking gain, temporal lag or tracking error between target and pointer trajectories are measurable evidence of these improvements (Huang and Hwang, 2012). Visuo-manual coordination in tracking tasks is often considered to be controlled by a combination of feed-forward and feedback adaptive internal models (Neilson et al, 1988;Miall et al, 1993;Shams and Seitz, 2008).…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…For example, Effenberg (Effenberg, 2004) proposed to use sound feedback to enhance movement perception. Kagerer and Contreras-Vidal describes particular phenomenons using auditory-motor transformations (Kagerer and Contreras-Vidal, 2009). They observed that the auditory-motor space can be affected by a newly formed internal model built consequently after a visuo-motor perturbation during a reaching task.…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Sosnoff et al [ 15 ] and Baweja et al [ 12 ] detected the effects of the level and frequency of display information on feedback control during force tracking tasks. Huang and Hwang explored the control pathway during tracking motions with or without visual information [ 13 ]. Gritsenko et al [ 6 ] and Lee et al [ 14 ] investigated the ability of adaption to the disturbance of the target jump or rotation during reaching tasks and confirmed the integration of both feedforward and feedback control.…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…3A, 3B), physically in accordance with the wider spectral spreads in high frequency of the force intermittency profile. Dynamic force-tracking in the shorter time scale was more informative, probably because the force tracking system adapted the required force output to multiple changing sensory inputs from the periphery to remedy tracking deviations in a short interval [33]. However, force intermittency of dynamic force-tracking in the high time scale 26–60 were conversely more regular (smaller MSE area) than those of static force-tracking (Figs.…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 97%
“…Under the framework of sampled movement control [10], [11], [12], force pulses in a force intermittency profile are centrally-scalable, superimposed onto the primary movement to tune a force trajectory [24], [26], [27]. Since dynamic tracking produced larger force intermittency and a smaller R PM/FI ratio than did static force-tracking (Table 1), dynamic force-tracking weighs more heavily on the error-correction process, entailing more intensive integration of proprioceptive and visual inputs than does static tracking [33]. Also, corrective adjustments to dynamic force-tracking were more frequent in order to generate motor commands in shorter time scales, on account of the higher number of high-frequency components with greater spectral dispersion in the force intermittency profile (Figs.…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 99%