“…Scalar implicature has long been one of the most studied phenomena in experimental pragmatics—indeed, the study of scalar implicature played a seminal role in the emergence of ‘experimental pragmatics’ as a field. Much research on the computation of scalar implicatures relies on behavioural measures, including explicit judgements about the meanings of various utterances under various conditions (Bott & Noveck, 2004; Bott, Bailey, & Grodner, 2012; Chemla & Spector, 2011; Chevallier et al, 2008; De Neys & Schaeken, 2007; Degen, 2015; Dieussaert, Verkerk, Gillard, & Schaeken, 2011; Doran, Baker, McNabb, Larson, & Ward, 2009; Feeney, Scafton, Duckworth, & Handley, 2004; Geurts & Pouscoulous, 2009; Goodman & Stuhlmüller, 2013; Marty & Chemla, 2013; Marty, Chemla, & Spector, 2013; van Tiel, van Miltenburg, Zevakhina, & Geurts, 2016; among others), reaction and reading time measures (Bergen & Grodner, 2012; Bezuidenhout & Cutting, 2002; Bott & Noveck, 2004; Breheny, Katsos, & Williams, 2006; Chemla, Cummins, & Singh, 2017; Hartshorne & Snedeker, 2014; Noveck & Posada, 2003; Politzer‐Ahles & Fiorentino, 2013; Politzer‐Ahles & Husband, 2018; among others), measures of eye movements (Breheny, Ferguson, & Katsos, 2012, 2013; Degen & Tanenhaus, 2015; Grodner, Klein, Carbary, & Tanenhaus, 2010; Huang & Snedeker, 2009; among others) and mouse tracking (Tomlinson, Bailey, & Bott, 2013). Recent reviews of these literatures are available in, for example, Chemla and Singh (2014a,b) and Sauerland and Schumacher (2016).…”