2020
DOI: 10.1002/aur.2432
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Eye Movements and Behavioural Responses to Gaze‐Contingent Expressive Faces in Typically Developing Infants and Infant Siblings

Abstract: Studies with infant siblings of children with Autism Spectrum Disorder have attempted to identify early markers for the disorder and suggest that autistic symptoms emerge between 12 and 24 months of age. Yet, a reliable first‐year marker remains elusive. We propose that in order to establish first‐year manifestations of this inherently social disorder, we need to develop research methods that are sufficiently socially demanding and realistically interactive. Building on Keemink et al. [2019, Developmental Psyc… Show more

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
4
1

Citation Types

0
12
0

Year Published

2021
2021
2021
2021

Publication Types

Select...
5

Relationship

2
3

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 5 publications
(12 citation statements)
references
References 73 publications
0
12
0
Order By: Relevance
“…This differs from everyday social interaction, where faces are encountered as three-dimensional and highly dynamic stimuli, in diverse and meaningful contexts, and rarely occur in isolation. Some studies have attempted to address this discrepancy between the laboratory and the real world by employing videos of faces (Hermens & Walker, 2012;Keemink et al, 2020;Lander et al, 2001;O'Toole et al, 2011) or by recruiting live confederates to act as stimuli (Kemp et al, 1997;Megreya & Burton, 2008;Ritchie et al, 2020;White et al, 2014), but such approaches come with their own limitations. For instance, while pre-recorded videos display dynamic faces, these representations are seldom interactive.…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…This differs from everyday social interaction, where faces are encountered as three-dimensional and highly dynamic stimuli, in diverse and meaningful contexts, and rarely occur in isolation. Some studies have attempted to address this discrepancy between the laboratory and the real world by employing videos of faces (Hermens & Walker, 2012;Keemink et al, 2020;Lander et al, 2001;O'Toole et al, 2011) or by recruiting live confederates to act as stimuli (Kemp et al, 1997;Megreya & Burton, 2008;Ritchie et al, 2020;White et al, 2014), but such approaches come with their own limitations. For instance, while pre-recorded videos display dynamic faces, these representations are seldom interactive.…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Previous work has established that infants are able to perceptually discriminate and categorise emotional expressions (Addabbo et al, 2018;Farroni et al, 2007;Keemink et al, 2019;Kotsoni et al, 2001;Nelson et al, 1979;Ruba et al, 2017;Safar et al, 2017). Evidence from eye-tracking paradigms has also shown that infants scan facial features in a similar way to adults; looking toward regions that are 'diagnostic' for decoding expressions (Hunnius et al, 2011;Keemink et al, 2021;Prunty et al, 2021;Soussignan et al, 2017). Yet how infants perceive facial expressions tells us little about whether or not they are receptive to the information communicated by expressive stimuli (Nelson, 1987;Ruba et al, 2019).…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Gaze-contingently activated videos also allow a standardization across trials and participants, as it ensures all infants are fixating the same location (eye-region) before the expression response begins, and guarantees their attentiveness. Similar paradigms have been used previously to simulate infant-adult social exchanges (Keemink et al, 2019;Vernetti et al, 2018), and recently to investigate infants' behavioral responses to facial expressions (Keemink et al, 2021). In this paradigm, real dynamic human faces (c.f.…”
Section: The Gaze-contingent Eye-tracking Paradigmmentioning
confidence: 99%