2002
DOI: 10.1017/s0140525x02000080
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Facial expression of pain: An evolutionary account

Abstract: The insights provided by the application of evolutionary psychology to established fields of evidence has clinical relevance in areas such as fears, anxiety, and depression (Dimberg & Ohman 1996; Gilbert 1992; Marks & Nesse 1994). Certain evolved behaviours, such as the attachment behaviour of human and other primate infants, are accepted as orthodoxy. But broader notions of an evolved "human nature," that is, of specialised propensities 1 that generate functional behaviours effective in the social and materia… Show more

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
2
1
1
1

Citation Types

16
404
0
8

Year Published

2011
2011
2022
2022

Publication Types

Select...
8

Relationship

1
7

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 331 publications
(428 citation statements)
references
References 317 publications
(170 reference statements)
16
404
0
8
Order By: Relevance
“…People can be reasonably successful in suppressing or exaggerating nonverbal pain expression (Crombez & Eccleston, 2002;Hadjistavropoulos, Craig, Hadjistavropoulos, & Poole, 1996a;Hill & Craig, 2002;Larochette et al, 2006;Vervoort et al, 2008b), in accordance with perception of situational demands. As indicated earlier, like other negative facial expressions, expression of pain tends to be inhibited in the presence of strangers, although this inhibition may be lifted when strangers are recognized as (professional) caregivers, potentially able to relieve pain (Williams, 2002). This evidence contrasts with the popular formulation that patients 'exaggerate' pain-related behavior in the presence of actual or potential (Coyne & Fiske, 1992;Lackner & Gurtman, 2004;Lyons, Sullivan, Ritvo, & Coyne, 1995;Taylor et al, 2000).…”
Section: Pain Communication 35mentioning
confidence: 94%
See 1 more Smart Citation
“…People can be reasonably successful in suppressing or exaggerating nonverbal pain expression (Crombez & Eccleston, 2002;Hadjistavropoulos, Craig, Hadjistavropoulos, & Poole, 1996a;Hill & Craig, 2002;Larochette et al, 2006;Vervoort et al, 2008b), in accordance with perception of situational demands. As indicated earlier, like other negative facial expressions, expression of pain tends to be inhibited in the presence of strangers, although this inhibition may be lifted when strangers are recognized as (professional) caregivers, potentially able to relieve pain (Williams, 2002). This evidence contrasts with the popular formulation that patients 'exaggerate' pain-related behavior in the presence of actual or potential (Coyne & Fiske, 1992;Lackner & Gurtman, 2004;Lyons, Sullivan, Ritvo, & Coyne, 1995;Taylor et al, 2000).…”
Section: Pain Communication 35mentioning
confidence: 94%
“…More specifically, responses to pain responses (i.e., both self-report and facial behavior as assessed by judges who viewed videos of the participants' responses) to electric shock were attenuated when subjects were being observed. Self-report tends to be exquisitely sensitive to the social context as perceived, with individuals typically using language to optimize situational outcomes and nonverbal expression is demonstrably sensitive to context in both adults and children (Crombez & Eccleston, 2002;Hadjistavropoulos & Craig, 2002;Kleck et al, 1976;Larochette et al, 2006;Prkachin & Craig, 1985;Sullivan et al, 2004;Vervoort et al, 2008b; Pain Communication 37 Williams, 2002;Zeman & Garber, 1996). Levine and De Simone (1991) showed that males report less experimentally-induced pain in the presence of an attractively dressed female experimenter than in the presence of a male.…”
Section: Pain Communication 35mentioning
confidence: 99%
“…The interactive teaching methods used in several studies fell short of capturing the challenges of nurses' everyday work, which involves shifts in attention, multi-tasking, ad hoc changes to priorities, and interruptions (Bragadóttir, Gunnarsdóttir, Ingason, 2014) These can push nurses towards discounting pain or estimating it from extraneous cues (age, sex, social class, ethnicity) rather than patient self-report (Williams, 2002). Contextual and relational factors have an important impact on pain management behaviors but are difficult to address solely by reference to misconceptions about pain or by training nurses on pain assessment instruments.…”
Section: Ravaud Et Al 2004)mentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Fifth, the coding system we used was limited to verbal behavior. Non-verbal behavior, however, is also an important feature of parent-child communication [42,82].…”
Section: " "mentioning
confidence: 99%
“…There is reason to believe that, in addition to parental characteristics (e.g., catastrophizing), contextual variables such as additional information concerning the situation [55,75] and child characteristics, such as facial pain expression [42,82] may be important in explaining variations in parental responses to child pain. These variables may augment the threat value parents assign to the child's pain experience (i.e., contextual threat), thereby enhancing parental distress and possibly pain-attending responses [5,6,20,67,70,82]. Accordingly, the present study additionally examined whether contextual information or child pain expression interacts with 4" " parental catastrophizing to impact parental emotional and behavioral responses to their child's pain.…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 99%