1981
DOI: 10.1121/1.385342
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Factors in the discrimination of tonal patterns. III. Frequency discrimination with components of well-learned patterns

Abstract: Estimates of the effectiveness of selective auditory attention were obtained by adjusting the level of a target tone, which was presented as one 40-msec component in a ten-tone sequential pattern. When the levels of target and nontarget tones were the same, frequency-discrimination thresholds (delta f/f) following prolonged training varied from 0.01-0.02 for minimal-uncertainty testing conditions to 0.1-0.2 under high trial-to-trial stimulus uncertainty. The functions relating frequency discrimination to targe… Show more

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
2
1

Citation Types

4
24
1

Year Published

1985
1985
2018
2018

Publication Types

Select...
6
2
1

Relationship

0
9

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 44 publications
(29 citation statements)
references
References 0 publications
4
24
1
Order By: Relevance
“…The elevation in thresholds due to a more complex stimulus has been termed informational masking (Pollack, 1975;Watson, 1987). Results from studies that have used 9-or 10-tone equal-length stimulus components consisting of tones randomly chosen from the frequency range within which most speech information falls (300-3000 Hz) are consistent with the idea that informational masking may impose a limit on performance (Spiegel & Watson, 1981;Watson, Kelly, & Wroton, 1976;Watson, Wroton, Kelly, & Benbasset, 1975). Watson et al (1990) presented sequences of pure tones in a frequency discrimination task and found a strong relationship between the number of components in a tonal sequence and the threshold for discriminating one component within a sequence.…”
Section: The Proportion-of-the-total-duration Rulesupporting
confidence: 61%
See 1 more Smart Citation
“…The elevation in thresholds due to a more complex stimulus has been termed informational masking (Pollack, 1975;Watson, 1987). Results from studies that have used 9-or 10-tone equal-length stimulus components consisting of tones randomly chosen from the frequency range within which most speech information falls (300-3000 Hz) are consistent with the idea that informational masking may impose a limit on performance (Spiegel & Watson, 1981;Watson, Kelly, & Wroton, 1976;Watson, Wroton, Kelly, & Benbasset, 1975). Watson et al (1990) presented sequences of pure tones in a frequency discrimination task and found a strong relationship between the number of components in a tonal sequence and the threshold for discriminating one component within a sequence.…”
Section: The Proportion-of-the-total-duration Rulesupporting
confidence: 61%
“…Human observers can detect a change in frequency of about 3% for frequencies up to about 1000 Hz (Harris, 1952) and can, in some cases, detect as little as a 0.2% change (Wier, Jesteadt, & Green, 1977). However, when the stimuli become more complex by the addition of context tones before and after the target, discrimination thresholds for a single target within a multi-tone sequence fall dramatically, often with a 20%-30% change or more being needed for consistent discrimination (Spiegel & Watson, 1981). This effect has led some researchers to suggest that the more complex a stimulus, the more its processing is determined by central rather than peripheral mechanisms (Watson & Kelly, 1981).…”
Section: The Proportion-of-the-total-duration Rulementioning
confidence: 99%
“…Watson and his colleagues (Spiegel & Watson, 1981;Watson & Kelly, 1981;Watson, Kelly, & Wroton, 1976;Watson, Wroton, Kelly, & Benbassat, 1975) investigated listeners' discrimination of complex word-length patterns consisting of ten 40-msec tonal components. Not surprisingly, performance in most conditions improved substantially with practice.…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…On the other hand, the number of publications devoted specifically to the influence of practice on perceptual auditory performance has remained surprisingly small in comparison with the large number of questions that are still unanswered in this area. Basically, leaving aside studies devoted to auditory learning in speech perception (e.g., Bradlow, Pisoni, AkahaneYamada, & Tohkura, 1997), only a few studies have been devoted specifically to perceptual auditory learning for detection (Gundy, 1961;Zwislocki, Maire, Feldman, & Rubin, 1958) and discrimination in frequency (Campbell & Small, 1963;Demany, 1985;Wyatt, 1945) or duration (Wright, Buonomano, Mahncke, & Merzenich, 1997) of pure tones presented either in isolation or as part of complex temporal sequences (Leek & Watson, 1984;Spiegel & Watson, 1981;Watson, Kelly, & Wroton 1976; for a reThe study described in this article received the approval of the ethics committee (CCPPRB/61-99). The subjects' written informed consent was obtained.…”
mentioning
confidence: 99%