PsycEXTRA Dataset 2012
DOI: 10.1037/e667222012-001
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Facts-Up-Front Versus Traffic-Light Food Labels: A Randomized Controlled Trial

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
1
1
1
1

Citation Types

1
30
0
4

Year Published

2014
2014
2020
2020

Publication Types

Select...
6
1
1

Relationship

0
8

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 32 publications
(43 citation statements)
references
References 0 publications
1
30
0
4
Order By: Relevance
“…the healthy choice) easier to make (Thaler & Sunstein, 2008). Examples of healthy eating nudges include placing fruit at the cash registry instead of candy bars, which increased fruit intake (Kroese, Marchiori, & De Ridder, 2016), implicitly signalling a social norm by displaying packaging of healthy snacks, influencing food choice between healthy and unhealthy food choices (Prinsen et al, 2013), traffic light labelling of foods, leading to increased nutrient knowledge as well as identification of health as an important factor in purchasing decisions (Roberto et al, 2012;Sonnenberg et al, 2013), and changing default portion sizes or choices, although results are mixed: one paper reports similar energy intake with smaller versus bigger plates, with bigger plates having the advantage of more vegetable sidedishes (Libotte, Siegrist, & Bucher, 2014) whereas another warns against the danger of overeating with big plates (Wansink & Van Ittersum, 2013).…”
Section: Physical Environmentmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…the healthy choice) easier to make (Thaler & Sunstein, 2008). Examples of healthy eating nudges include placing fruit at the cash registry instead of candy bars, which increased fruit intake (Kroese, Marchiori, & De Ridder, 2016), implicitly signalling a social norm by displaying packaging of healthy snacks, influencing food choice between healthy and unhealthy food choices (Prinsen et al, 2013), traffic light labelling of foods, leading to increased nutrient knowledge as well as identification of health as an important factor in purchasing decisions (Roberto et al, 2012;Sonnenberg et al, 2013), and changing default portion sizes or choices, although results are mixed: one paper reports similar energy intake with smaller versus bigger plates, with bigger plates having the advantage of more vegetable sidedishes (Libotte, Siegrist, & Bucher, 2014) whereas another warns against the danger of overeating with big plates (Wansink & Van Ittersum, 2013).…”
Section: Physical Environmentmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Indeed, consumers demand a simplified standard with clearer and more comprehensible indications (Besler et al 2012). Consumer understanding of labels was also affected by the presence of FoPs (Ares et al 2012), which is evaluated positively if in graphical format (Geiger et al 1991), such as the traffic light for instance (Roberto et al 2012 information improve consumer comprehension of nutrient content in foodstuffs. However, the existence of multiple FoP formats limits consumer comprehension and discourages their use (Draper et al 2013).…”
Section: Results On Consumer Understanding Of Nutrition Informationmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…La plupart des autres études sur les logos nutritionnels sont des enquêtes par questionnaires. Ces études, qui portent sur l'avis des consommateurs sur les logos, l'usage qu'ils déclarent en faire et la compréhension qu'ils en ont, tendent à montrer que les consommateurs sont sensibles aux logos nutritionnels et les accueillent favorablement [16][17][18]. Ainsi, une étude basée sur un questionnaire délivré par Internet dans 4 pays européens (n'incluant pas la France) conclut que les consommateurs apprécient de façon similaire tous les logos face-avant testés, les comprennent plutôt bien et seraient capables de distinguer grâce à eux les aliments sains des moins sains [19].…”
Section: Discussionunclassified