2016
DOI: 10.1037/pspi0000045
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Failure at the top: How power undermines collaborative performance.

Abstract: All too commonly, we see groups of leaders fail to accomplish their stated goals when working together -legislators who cannot agree on a bill, heads of state who cannot draft meaningful environmental policy, or boards of trustees who make disastrous decisions for their school. The current research examines whether groups of leaders fail as often as they do in part because of the power each leader is accustomed to possessing. Multiple studies found high power individuals, when working in groups, performed wors… Show more

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
2
1
1

Citation Types

1
40
0

Year Published

2016
2016
2021
2021

Publication Types

Select...
7
1

Relationship

0
8

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 38 publications
(41 citation statements)
references
References 148 publications
(229 reference statements)
1
40
0
Order By: Relevance
“…This hypothesis requires direct testing in future research, but there is some indirect evidence supporting it. For example, dyads and groups with too many high-status individuals have impaired collective performance on tasks requiring coordination due to increased intragroup conflict, whereas status differentiation improves collective performance by improving coordination and reducing conflict (Ronay et al, 2012;Swaab et al, 2014;Wiltermuth et al, 2015;Hildreth and Anderson, 2016;Kilduff et al, 2016). Furthermore, groups in which high-testosterone individuals hold high-status positions and low-testosterone individuals hold low-status positions enjoy high levels of group self-efficacy, a psychological factor that may be involved in group coordination and productivity (Zyphur et al, 2009).…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 96%
“…This hypothesis requires direct testing in future research, but there is some indirect evidence supporting it. For example, dyads and groups with too many high-status individuals have impaired collective performance on tasks requiring coordination due to increased intragroup conflict, whereas status differentiation improves collective performance by improving coordination and reducing conflict (Ronay et al, 2012;Swaab et al, 2014;Wiltermuth et al, 2015;Hildreth and Anderson, 2016;Kilduff et al, 2016). Furthermore, groups in which high-testosterone individuals hold high-status positions and low-testosterone individuals hold low-status positions enjoy high levels of group self-efficacy, a psychological factor that may be involved in group coordination and productivity (Zyphur et al, 2009).…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 96%
“…For example, when the hierarchy was unstable, dominant leaders were inclined to exclude threatening group members ( Maner & Mead, 2010 ). Also, when put in teams, leaders competed over position in a newly formed group, thus undermining collaboration and team performance ( Hildreth & Anderson, 2016 ). It is then possible that a tenuous hold of a high position decreases personal control, while strengthening the need to assert power, resulting in yet stronger negative interpersonal consequences.…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Furthermore, power can decrease judgment accuracy when power holders are overconfident or not motivated, which has been documented in the social domain (Fiske & Berdahl 2007, Nissan et al 2015. Finally, when people in power work together in panels and committees, they often have conflicts and their individual (as well as the group's) performance deteriorates (Hildreth & Anderson 2016). To conclude, there is a power advantage in performance across many contexts and tasks, but the links between power and performance are nuanced and depend on the task and the motivation to complete it.…”
Section: Does Power Enhance Performance?mentioning
confidence: 99%