2018
DOI: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2018.07.017
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Fast and slow thinking: Electrophysiological evidence for early conflict sensitivity

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
2
1
1

Citation Types

4
18
0

Year Published

2018
2018
2023
2023

Publication Types

Select...
8
1

Relationship

1
8

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 37 publications
(24 citation statements)
references
References 53 publications
4
18
0
Order By: Relevance
“…Our findings add to a growing body of EEG and fMRI literature [11][12][13][14] investigating the neural basis of reasoning. Our results are also in line with studies examining cognitive control and executive functioning that have probed the underlying mechanisms of reasoning by employing niche tasks to assess specific theorized mechanisms 27 .…”
Section: Discussionsupporting
confidence: 58%
See 1 more Smart Citation
“…Our findings add to a growing body of EEG and fMRI literature [11][12][13][14] investigating the neural basis of reasoning. Our results are also in line with studies examining cognitive control and executive functioning that have probed the underlying mechanisms of reasoning by employing niche tasks to assess specific theorized mechanisms 27 .…”
Section: Discussionsupporting
confidence: 58%
“…To address this gap, in the current study we used electroencephalography to probe the neural correlates of human rationality. We accomplished this by having participants complete base-rate problems (e.g., Bago et al 11 ) so that we could examine whether neural oscillations are sensitive to intuitive and rational reasoning 15 . Furthermore, neural oscillations are theorized to correspond to the degree of communication across the brain -fast oscillations reflect localized brain activity and slow oscillations reflect the coordination of structures across a diverse brain network 16 .…”
mentioning
confidence: 99%
“…This brings up the question of hybrid models (Bago & De Neys, 2017; Banks, 2017; De Neys, 2012; Epstein, 1994; Handley & Trippas, 2015; Johnson et al, 2016; Nakamura & Kawaguchi, 2016; Pennycook et al, 2014, 2015; Sloman, 1996; Smith & DeCoster, 2000; Thompson & Johnson, 2014; Trippas et al, 2016, 2017; Trippas & Handley, 2017). Bago et al (2018) developed a hybrid dual-process model in which the faster System 1 thinking cues both heuristic, belief-based responses (stereotypes) and elementary logico-mathematical principles (base rates). Julmi (2019) also took this approach, adopting a parallel-competitive view based on growing evidence from neurological and experimental studies (Alós-Ferrer & Strack, 2014; Healey et al, 2015; Kuo et al, 2009; Lieberman, 2007).…”
Section: Theoretical Frameworkmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…This is, however, not what the available evidence suggests. A large body of conflict detection studies shows that when people give heuristic responses on conflict versions of reasoning problems, they show decreased response confidence in comparison with the noconflict versions (Frey & De Neys, 2017;Mevel et al, 2014;Stupple, Ball, & Ellis, 2013), prolonged response times (Pennycook et al, 2015;Stupple & Ball, 2008;Swan, Calvillo, & Revlin, 2018), lower feelings of rightness about their answers (Thompson & Johnson, 2014), better recall of information presented in the task (De Neys & Glumicic, 2008), changes in skin conductance (De Neys, Moyens, & Vansteenwegen, 2010), and other neurophysiological changes (Bago et al, 2018;De Neys, Vartanian, & Goel, 2008;Vartanian et al, 2018). This seems to indicate that even when people are biased, they are at least implicitly sensitive to the fact that their response is not in line with the logically correct response (De Neys, 2012, 2017 however, for critics of this account see Mata, Ferreira, Voss, & Kollei, 2017;Pennycook, Fugelsang, & Koehler, 2012;Singmann, Klauer, & Kellen, 2014).…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 99%