2019
DOI: 10.1177/0956797619852027
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Fear Without Context: Acute Stress Modulates the Balance of Cue-Dependent and Contextual Fear Learning

Abstract: During a threatening encounter, people can learn to associate the aversive event with a discrete preceding cue or with the context in which the event took place, corresponding to cue-dependent and context-dependent fear conditioning, respectively. Which of these forms of fear learning prevails has critical implications for fear-related psychopathology. We tested here whether acute stress may modulate the balance of cue-dependent and contextual fear learning. Participants ( N = 72) underwent a stress or control… Show more

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
2
2

Citation Types

0
17
0

Year Published

2019
2019
2023
2023

Publication Types

Select...
4
3
1

Relationship

2
6

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 33 publications
(17 citation statements)
references
References 37 publications
0
17
0
Order By: Relevance
“…8,[32][33][34]41 Possibly, we were not able to demonstrate the fast and immediate effect of acute stress in the current study, due to the relatively long delay between the acute stressor and fear acquisition (Figure 3). In the present study, fear contextualization was measured between 40 and 70 min after acute stress exposure onset (in the immediate-stress group), which is later than in earlier published studies 8,[33][34][35] and slightly overlaps with potential delayed effects. This is a limitation of our design (see "No Time-Dependent Effect of Psychosocial Stress on Fear Contextualization" section) which was optimized to investigate the delayed effects of stress, since these (as opposed to immediate actions) are heavily understudied.…”
Section: No Time-dependent Effect Of Psychosocial Stress On Fear Contmentioning
confidence: 85%
See 2 more Smart Citations
“…8,[32][33][34]41 Possibly, we were not able to demonstrate the fast and immediate effect of acute stress in the current study, due to the relatively long delay between the acute stressor and fear acquisition (Figure 3). In the present study, fear contextualization was measured between 40 and 70 min after acute stress exposure onset (in the immediate-stress group), which is later than in earlier published studies 8,[33][34][35] and slightly overlaps with potential delayed effects. This is a limitation of our design (see "No Time-Dependent Effect of Psychosocial Stress on Fear Contextualization" section) which was optimized to investigate the delayed effects of stress, since these (as opposed to immediate actions) are heavily understudied.…”
Section: No Time-dependent Effect Of Psychosocial Stress On Fear Contmentioning
confidence: 85%
“…This would be in line with the findings of Antov et al in a cued-fear conditioning paradigm, 58 but contrasts with earlier studies that identified a direct relation between context-dependency of emotional information and cortisol-responses following hydrocortisone administration 8,31 or psychosocial stress. 35 Importantly, these studies used different tasks to measure contextdependency of emotional information, including an episodic memory task 31 and fear conditioning tasks with skin conductance responses (SCR) 8,35,58 and FPS responses 8 as outcome measures. Since emotional episodic memory involves a different neurocircuit than fear conditioning 61 and SCR reflects different dimensions of fear learning than FPS responses, 62 it is likely that these tasks are also differentially affected by stress or cortisol.…”
Section: No Time-dependent Effect Of Psychosocial Stress On Fear Contmentioning
confidence: 99%
See 1 more Smart Citation
“…Two of the most prominent stress mediators are glucocorticoids and noradrenaline, both of which are known to be major modulators of learning and memory in general (Diamond, Campbell, Park, Halonen, & Zoladz, 2007 ; Joels, Fernandez, & Roozendaal, 2011 ; Quaedflieg & Schwabe, 2018 ; Roozendaal, Okuda, de Quervain, & McGaugh, 2006 ; Sandi & Pinelo-Nava, 2007 ; Schwabe, Joels, Roozendaal, Wolf, & Oitzl, 2012 ). Moreover, there also is evidence that stress affects fear learning processes (Jackson, Payne, Nadel, & Jacobs, 2006 ; Merz, Elzinga, & Schwabe, 2016 ; Simon-Kutscher, Wanke, Hiller, & Schwabe, 2019 ), presumably also driven by glucocorticoids and noradrenergic arousal (Krugers, Zhou, Joels, & Kindt, 2011 ; Merz, Hamacher-Dang, Stark, Wolf, & Hermann, 2018 ). Prefrontal and medial-temporal brain areas critically involved in fear generalization (Greenberg et al, 2013 ; Lissek, Bradford, et al, 2014 ; Lopresto et al, 2016 ; Onat & Büchel, 2015 ) are known to be particularly sensitive to stress and stress mediators (de Kloet, Joels, & Holsboer, 2005 ; Krugers, Karst, & Joels, 2012 ; Roozendaal et al, 2006 ).…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Two participants did not return for the second experimental day and were excluded from the analysis. The target sample size was based on experience from previous studies on stress and memory processes(Schönfeld, Ackermann, & Schwabe, 2014;Simon-Kutscher, Wanke, Hiller, & Schwabe, 2019;Zerbes & Schwabe, 2019). While no prior research targeted retroactive memory enhancements specifically through social stress, our research design in this experiment was inspired by the study byDunsmoor et al (2015).…”
mentioning
confidence: 99%