2016
DOI: 10.1371/journal.pone.0151188
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Feature Integration and Task Switching: Diminished Switch Costs after Controlling for Stimulus, Response, and Cue Repetitions

Abstract: This report presents data from two versions of the task switching procedure in which the separate influence of stimulus repetitions, response key repetitions, conceptual response repetitions, cue repetitions, task repetitions, and congruency are considered. Experiment 1 used a simple alternating runs procedure with parity judgments of digits and consonant/vowel decisions of letters as the two tasks. Results revealed sizable effects of stimulus and response repetitions, and controlling for these effects reduced… Show more

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
2
1
1
1

Citation Types

1
55
0

Year Published

2016
2016
2023
2023

Publication Types

Select...
7

Relationship

1
6

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 36 publications
(56 citation statements)
references
References 75 publications
1
55
0
Order By: Relevance
“…However, both effects have been argued to be systematically biased by exactly the sorts of learning biases discussed in the current report (see Schmidt, 2013b;Schmidt, Notebaert, & Van den Bussche, 2015). For instance, proportion congruent effects are often biased by contingencies (Hazeltine & Mordkoff, 2014;Schmidt, 2013a;Schmidt & Besner, 2008) and/or rhythmic response biases (Kinoshita et al, 2011;Schmidt, 2013c).…”
Section: Conflict Monitoring and Attentional Adaptationmentioning
confidence: 65%
See 3 more Smart Citations
“…However, both effects have been argued to be systematically biased by exactly the sorts of learning biases discussed in the current report (see Schmidt, 2013b;Schmidt, Notebaert, & Van den Bussche, 2015). For instance, proportion congruent effects are often biased by contingencies (Hazeltine & Mordkoff, 2014;Schmidt, 2013a;Schmidt & Besner, 2008) and/or rhythmic response biases (Kinoshita et al, 2011;Schmidt, 2013c).…”
Section: Conflict Monitoring and Attentional Adaptationmentioning
confidence: 65%
“…Future work might aim to explore whether our simple learning account is sufficient, or whether attentional adaptation to conflict plays an additional role in these phenomena. This is already a heated debate in the attentional control literature (Atalay & Misirlisoy, 2012;Bugg, Jacoby, & Chanani, 2011;Crump & Milliken, 2009;Hazeltine & Mordkoff, 2014;Levin & Tzelgov, 2016;Notebaert & Verguts, 2007;Schmidt, De Schryver, & Weissman, 2014;Schmidt et al, 2015) and the PEP model might serve as a useful reference for how far one can go with episodic learning alone.…”
Section: Conflict Monitoring and Attentional Adaptationmentioning
confidence: 99%
See 2 more Smart Citations
“…The current work, and that of Grange et al (2017), sits within a broader context of work demonstrating a role of episodic memory retrieval in explaining (or at least, contributing to) key effects in the task switching literature. For example, work has shown a considerable contribution of episodic (and semantic) memory retrieval to the task switch cost-the observed slowing of RTs on task switch trials compared to task repetition trials-in the explicitly-cued task switching paradigm (Altmann & Gray, 2008;Logan, 2003;Schmidt & Liefooghe, 2016;Schneider & Logan, 2005). The observed reduction of the task switch cost with inreasing RCI-once attributed to the time-based decay of task-set activation (Meiran, Chorev, & Sapir, 2000)-has been shown to be attributable to temporal distinctiveness effects during automatic, cue-based retrieval of task-sets from episodic memory (Grange, 2016;Grange & Cross, 2015;Horoufchin et al, 2011).…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 99%