2021
DOI: 10.1016/j.brat.2021.103973
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Feeling safe: Judgements of safety and anxiety as a function of worry and intolerance of uncertainty

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
2
2
1

Citation Types

0
3
0

Year Published

2023
2023
2024
2024

Publication Types

Select...
5
4

Relationship

0
9

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 15 publications
(13 citation statements)
references
References 45 publications
0
3
0
Order By: Relevance
“…There is some evidence that individuals with GAD do not discriminate well between stimuli that are associated with a negative outcome (e.g., a shock) and stimuli associated with a safe outcome (e.g., no shock), which suggests there may be deficits in safety learning (e.g., Lissek et al, 2005;Lissek et al, 2009). However, although Cupid et al (2021) found that greater chronic worry was associated with lower perception of safety in both safe and potentially threatening scenarios, individuals higher in worry did distinguish safe scenarios as relatively safe compared to the threatening scenarios. Thus, further work is warranted in this area in order to understand whether uncertain scenarios that are likely to end positively are actually recognized as likely to end well by individuals higher in worry.…”
Section: Outcome Valencementioning
confidence: 69%
See 1 more Smart Citation
“…There is some evidence that individuals with GAD do not discriminate well between stimuli that are associated with a negative outcome (e.g., a shock) and stimuli associated with a safe outcome (e.g., no shock), which suggests there may be deficits in safety learning (e.g., Lissek et al, 2005;Lissek et al, 2009). However, although Cupid et al (2021) found that greater chronic worry was associated with lower perception of safety in both safe and potentially threatening scenarios, individuals higher in worry did distinguish safe scenarios as relatively safe compared to the threatening scenarios. Thus, further work is warranted in this area in order to understand whether uncertain scenarios that are likely to end positively are actually recognized as likely to end well by individuals higher in worry.…”
Section: Outcome Valencementioning
confidence: 69%
“…Recently, Cupid et al (2021) found that individuals higher in IU had less of a difference in their degree of anxiety in response to "safe" and "potentially threatening" uncertain scenarios than did individuals lower in IU. Further they found that the relationship between IU and anxiety was greater in the relatively safe scenarios than the potentially threatening scenarios, suggesting that pathology in IU may lie in reactions to relatively safe uncertainty.…”
Section: Outcome Valencementioning
confidence: 99%
“…As a result of the isolation process, which is an inseparable part of the measures taken against the COVID-19 pandemic, many psychological and medical problems have arisen 60 . During this period, it can be said that individuals with high levels of intolerance to uncertainty and anxiety struggle with accepting the security measures taken 76 .…”
Section: Impact On Behaviorsmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Intolerance of Uncertainty (IU) is the tendency to find uncertainty distressing (Carleton, 2016a;Freeston, Rhéaume, Letarte, Dugas, & Ladouceur, 1994). Previous research has demonstrated that individuals with higher IU interpret uncertain scenarios as more threatening (Cupid, Stewart, Sumantry, & Koerner, 2021;Pepperdine, Lomax, & Freeston, 2018), and under uncertainty they tend to experience greater negative affect (Morriss, Goh, Hirsch, & Dodd, 2023) and physiological arousal (Morriss, Zuj, & Mertens, 2021;Tanovic, Gee, & Joormann, 2018). IU is commonly measured using the 12-item IU scale, which can be used as unilateral scale or split into two subscales (Carleton et al, 2007;Hong & Lee, 2015).…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 99%