2020
DOI: 10.1002/ecs2.3051
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Fitting functional response surfaces to data: a best practice guide

Abstract: Describing how resource consumption rates depend on resource density, conventionally termed "functional responses," is crucial to understanding the population dynamics of trophically interacting organisms. Yet, accurately determining the functional response for any given pair of predator and prey remains a challenge. Moreover, functional responses are potentially complex surfaces in multidimensional space, where resource density is only one of several factors determining consumption rates. We explored how thre… Show more

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
4
1

Citation Types

4
54
0

Year Published

2020
2020
2023
2023

Publication Types

Select...
6
2

Relationship

0
8

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 29 publications
(58 citation statements)
references
References 49 publications
4
54
0
Order By: Relevance
“…how many and how large a range of species abundance levels are employed; Sarnelle &Wilson (2008); Uszko et al . (2020)), observer error in estimating species densities and the number of eaten prey (Jost and Arditi, 2000), and the behavioural variation that exists among consumer and prey individuals (Chesson, 1984; Abrams, 2010). Many such sources of variation will be present even in the well‐controlled laboratory studies that dominate the literature, and could even be magnified in such studies given the small numbers of individual animals that are typically involved at low‐abundance treatment levels (Coblentz, 2020; Novak et al ., 2017).…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 99%
See 2 more Smart Citations
“…how many and how large a range of species abundance levels are employed; Sarnelle &Wilson (2008); Uszko et al . (2020)), observer error in estimating species densities and the number of eaten prey (Jost and Arditi, 2000), and the behavioural variation that exists among consumer and prey individuals (Chesson, 1984; Abrams, 2010). Many such sources of variation will be present even in the well‐controlled laboratory studies that dominate the literature, and could even be magnified in such studies given the small numbers of individual animals that are typically involved at low‐abundance treatment levels (Coblentz, 2020; Novak et al ., 2017).…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…This will occur directly via the bias inherent in their own maximum‐likelihood estimators (Box 2), and indirectly through their functional relationships and data‐dependent covariances with the estimates of other biased parameters. For example, bias is also expected for the attack rate parameter with which the interference parameter is functionally intertwined in the Arditi–Akçakaya model (see also Gutenkunst et al ., 2007; Uszko et al ., 2020), or for any deterministic functional‐response parameter when the assumed likelihood model entails `nuisance’ parameters whose estimates are biased (e.g. the standard deviation of the Gaussian likelihood; see Supplementary Materials ).…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 99%
See 1 more Smart Citation
“…More specifically, both model-comparison bias (Box 1) and parameter-estimation bias (Box 2) are a function of a focal model’s parametric and geometric complexity, how well these are estimated, and for consumer functional responses, how these interact with the feeding rate variation within and among the treatment levels of an experiment. Sources of variation will include experimental design (e.g., how many and how large a range of species abundance levels are employed; Sarnelle & Wilson (2008); Uszko et al (2020)), observer error in estimating species densities and the number of eaten prey (Jost & Arditi, 2000), and the behavioural variation that exists among consumer and prey individuals (Abrams, 2010; Chesson, 1984). Many such sources of variation will be present even in the well-controlled laboratory studies that dominate the literature, and could even be magnified in such studies given the small numbers of individual animals that are typically involved at low-abundance treatment levels (Coblentz & DeLong, 2020; Novak et al , 2017).…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…This is repeated across all the prey densities multiple times typically using different predator individuals for each trial. The experimenters then fit a functional response model (or models) to the data using one of several existing statistical methods depending on whether prey were replaced as they were eaten (Royama, 1971;Rogers, 1972;Bolker, 2008;Rosenbaum & Rall, 2018;Uszko, Diehl, & Wickman, 2020).…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 99%