2020
DOI: 10.1093/jole/lzaa009
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Flexibility in wild infant chimpanzee vocal behavior

Abstract: How did human language evolve from earlier forms of communication? One way to address this question is to compare prelinguistic human vocal behavior with nonhuman primate calls. An important finding has been that, prior to speech and from early on, human infant vocal behavior exhibits functional flexibility, or the capacity to produce sounds that are not tied to one specific function. This is reflected in human infants’ use of single categories of protophones (precursors of speech sounds) in various affective … Show more

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
1
1
1
1

Citation Types

1
16
0

Year Published

2020
2020
2024
2024

Publication Types

Select...
8
2

Relationship

3
7

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 15 publications
(17 citation statements)
references
References 83 publications
1
16
0
Order By: Relevance
“…For example, bonobos ( Pan paniscus ) produce ‘peep’ calls across several contexts, encompassing various emotional valences [15]. The same has been found for the ‘grunt’ calls in juvenile chimpanzees ( Pan troglodytes ) [16] and ‘coo’ calls in different macaque species [17–19].…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 80%
“…For example, bonobos ( Pan paniscus ) produce ‘peep’ calls across several contexts, encompassing various emotional valences [15]. The same has been found for the ‘grunt’ calls in juvenile chimpanzees ( Pan troglodytes ) [16] and ‘coo’ calls in different macaque species [17–19].…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 80%
“…Regarding laugh vocalizations, it seems reasonable to conclude that such emancipation took place only after the last common ancestor of extant great apes and humans existed, because ape laughter is bioacoustically distinct (Davila-Ross et al, 2009 ; Taylor et al, 2021 ) and closely linked to play (Matsusaka, 2004 ; Davila-Ross et al, 2011 ), so that there cannot be variants with similar acoustic properties in other behavioral contexts. Furthermore, human infants within their first year of life produce various speech-related vocal types (protophones) free from contexts, but not laughter (Oller et al, 2013 ; for a comparative approach, see Dezecache et al, 2020 ). Consequently, laughter must have been used more flexibly closer toward humans, occurring in a wide range of everyday social interactions with gradually modifying acoustic properties (Owren and Bachorowski, 2003 ; Davila-Ross et al, 2010 ), perhaps accompanying key changes in language evolution (for laughter in conversation, see Vettin and Todt, 2004 ; Flamson and Bryant, 2013 ), when it also became an expression of other motivations and functions.…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Despite overlapping contexts, play vocalizations and open-mouth faces are at least to some extent likely to differ in function. Play vocalizations seem to be more limited to the context of play than open-mouth faces ([ 84 ]; for functional flexibility, see [ 103 , 104 ]). Although open-mouth faces occur predominantly during play bouts, they have also been observed shortly prior to them in order to invite a conspecific to play [ 105 ].…”
Section: Social Use Of Open-mouth Faces Laughter and Other Play Vocal...mentioning
confidence: 99%