2017
DOI: 10.1093/cercor/bhx288
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Focal Suppression of Distractor Sounds by Selective Attention in Auditory Cortex

Abstract: Auditory selective attention is required for parsing crowded acoustic environments, but cortical systems mediating the influence of behavioral state on auditory perception are not well characterized. Previous neurophysiological studies suggest that attention produces a general enhancement of neural responses to important target sounds versus irrelevant distractors. However, behavioral studies suggest that in the presence of masking noise, attention provides a focal suppression of distractors that compete with … Show more

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
1
1
1
1

Citation Types

4
43
3

Year Published

2019
2019
2024
2024

Publication Types

Select...
6
2
1

Relationship

0
9

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 41 publications
(50 citation statements)
references
References 81 publications
(156 reference statements)
4
43
3
Order By: Relevance
“…An absence of similar changes in anaesthetized rats is consistent with anaesthesia-related partial blockade of top-down influences, as previously suggested by an absence of perceptual learning and cortical disconnectivity with anaesthesia (Aberg et al 2009;Jordan et al 2013;Mashour, 2014). These findings are in agreement with recent work showing context and experience-based predictions in humans and animals (Ostroff et al 2003;Fakhri et al 2012;Leung et al 2013;Skoe et al 2014;Sohoglu & Chait, 2016;Parras et al 2017;Schwartz & David, 2018). Furthermore, repetition-induced increases in speech understanding, as well as improvements in the visual recognition of degraded objects with repetition, provide evidence in support of our interpretation of an increased response to degraded predictable stimuli (Rivenez et al 2006;Muller et al 2013;Helfer et al 2018).…”
Section: Effects Of Ageing On Central Auditory System and Top-down Prsupporting
confidence: 91%
“…An absence of similar changes in anaesthetized rats is consistent with anaesthesia-related partial blockade of top-down influences, as previously suggested by an absence of perceptual learning and cortical disconnectivity with anaesthesia (Aberg et al 2009;Jordan et al 2013;Mashour, 2014). These findings are in agreement with recent work showing context and experience-based predictions in humans and animals (Ostroff et al 2003;Fakhri et al 2012;Leung et al 2013;Skoe et al 2014;Sohoglu & Chait, 2016;Parras et al 2017;Schwartz & David, 2018). Furthermore, repetition-induced increases in speech understanding, as well as improvements in the visual recognition of degraded objects with repetition, provide evidence in support of our interpretation of an increased response to degraded predictable stimuli (Rivenez et al 2006;Muller et al 2013;Helfer et al 2018).…”
Section: Effects Of Ageing On Central Auditory System and Top-down Prsupporting
confidence: 91%
“…Recent work suggests that mice are able to attend to explicitly cued visual 290 patterns (Wang and Krauzlis, 2018) or auditory streams (Chapuis and 291 Chadderton, 2018), indicating that mice may not lack a mechanism for top-down 292 attentional control. Both humans and carnivores (Schwartz and David, 2018) can 293 be cued implicitly using target probability -providing further evidence that the 294 effect of stimulus probability on perception may be due to sensory ecology rather 295 than taxonomy. While both primates and carnivores might use their auditory 296 senses to tune in and follow potential prey or conspecific communication signals, 297 tracking of the statistics in mice -a prey species -may predominantly serve to 298 detect sudden, potentially dangerous changes in the environment.…”
Section: Sensory Ecology 283mentioning
confidence: 98%
“…A possible explanation for 124 mice not taking advantage of tracking probabilities is that they are not able to focus 125 on a single frequency band in a continuous noise background with very sparse 126 tones appearing at random times. We reasoned that a more natural situation could 127 be the presence of multiple streams of tones that allow to attach selective attention 128 to one of these streams (Lakatos et al, 2013;Schwartz and David, 2018). We 129 therefore designed an experiment in which the animals had to detect a frequency 130 change in either one of two continuous streams of tone pips (Fig.…”
Section: Experiments 2: Frequency Change Detection In Streams 122mentioning
confidence: 99%
“…active conditions and between attend-toward vs. attend-away conditions (Niwa et al, 2012b;Schwartz and David, 2017;von Trapp et al, 2016).…”
Section: Average A1 Neurons' Feature Sensitivity Is Constant Across Amentioning
confidence: 99%