1998
DOI: 10.1016/s0163-6383(98)90014-5
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Following the direction of gaze and language development in 6-month-olds

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
1
1
1
1

Citation Types

8
168
1
2

Year Published

2000
2000
2024
2024

Publication Types

Select...
5
5

Relationship

1
9

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 251 publications
(179 citation statements)
references
References 8 publications
8
168
1
2
Order By: Relevance
“…For example, Mundy, Kasari, Sigman, and Ruskin (1995) found that the child's ability to respond to JA bids (point following) and social interaction skills (eliciting attention or engaging in turn-taking games) was predictive of later receptive language (r = 0.70 and 0.56) and somewhat more weakly to expressive language (r = 0.49 and 0.55). The predictive relation between responding to JA and receptive language has been replicated by other researchers (Morales, Mundy, & Rojas, 1998;Morales et al, 2000;Mundy & Gomes, 1998;Ulvund & Smith, 1996). In addition, Ulvund and Smith (1996) found that children's initiating of JA bids at 13 months could predict both receptive and expressive language at three years, and Mundy and Gomes (1998) reported a prediction from initiating JA at 16 months to both expressive and receptive language two months later.…”
Section: Early Social and Communicative Skillsmentioning
confidence: 63%
“…For example, Mundy, Kasari, Sigman, and Ruskin (1995) found that the child's ability to respond to JA bids (point following) and social interaction skills (eliciting attention or engaging in turn-taking games) was predictive of later receptive language (r = 0.70 and 0.56) and somewhat more weakly to expressive language (r = 0.49 and 0.55). The predictive relation between responding to JA and receptive language has been replicated by other researchers (Morales, Mundy, & Rojas, 1998;Morales et al, 2000;Mundy & Gomes, 1998;Ulvund & Smith, 1996). In addition, Ulvund and Smith (1996) found that children's initiating of JA bids at 13 months could predict both receptive and expressive language at three years, and Mundy and Gomes (1998) reported a prediction from initiating JA at 16 months to both expressive and receptive language two months later.…”
Section: Early Social and Communicative Skillsmentioning
confidence: 63%
“…Consistent with this view, an extensive literature suggests that people are very sensitive to where others are looking and rapidly and reflexively orient attention toward the place where the gaze of another is directed (see Frischen, Bayliss, & Tipper, 2007, for a recent review). In addition, this ability develops early in childhood (Hood, Willen, & Driver, 1998) and has been suggested to underpin more complex forms of social communication, such as vocabulary learning (Morales, Mundy, & Rojas, 1998) and theory of mind (Charman et al, 2000). As a result, it has been suggested that eye gaze may be a particularly strong cue to attention (Kuhn & Kingstone, 2009).…”
mentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Eye-gaze following at 6 months has been shown to correlate with vocabulary size at 18 months (Morales et al, 2000;Morales, Mundy, & Rojas, 1998) and in noun learning, children can use eye-gaze, head posture and gesture to infer speakers' referential intention (e.g., Baldwin, 1991;Carpenter , Akhtar & Tomasello, 1998;Gergely, Bekkering & Király, 2002;Woodward & Sommerville, 2000). Nappa, Wessel, McEldoon, Gleitman & Trueswell (2009) showed that 3-, 4-and 5-year-olds used the eye-gaze of the speaker to infer the meaning of novel relational verbs (of the type chase vs. flee) in linguistically uninformative contexts (e.g., He's mooping him).…”
Section: The Development Of Subject Knowledge Attention and Eye-gazementioning
confidence: 99%