1987
DOI: 10.1007/bf00292166
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Foraging behaviour and echolocation in the rufous horseshoe bat (Rhinolophus rouxi) of Sri Lanka

Abstract: In October 1984 foraging areas and foraging behaviour of the rufous horseshoe bat, Rhinolophus rouxi, were studied around a nursery colony on the hill slopes of Sri Lanka. The bats only foraged in dense forest and were not found in open woodlands (Fig. 1). This strongly supports the hypothesis that detection of fluttering prey is by pure tone echolocation within or close to echo-cluttering foliage. During a first activity period after sunset for about 30-60 min, the bats mainly caught insects on the wing. This… Show more

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
2
1
1

Citation Types

13
148
1

Year Published

1991
1991
2017
2017

Publication Types

Select...
5
3

Relationship

0
8

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 175 publications
(162 citation statements)
references
References 26 publications
13
148
1
Order By: Relevance
“…A typical horseshoe bat call is depicted in Figure 4a. It usually begins with a brief upward frequency modulation, followed by the characteristic long constant-frequency component, and terminates with a short downward frequency-modulated portion (Schnitzler, 1968;Neuweiler et al, 1987). Injection of MUS (Fig.…”
Section: Effects Of Gabaergic Drugsmentioning
confidence: 99%
See 1 more Smart Citation
“…A typical horseshoe bat call is depicted in Figure 4a. It usually begins with a brief upward frequency modulation, followed by the characteristic long constant-frequency component, and terminates with a short downward frequency-modulated portion (Schnitzler, 1968;Neuweiler et al, 1987). Injection of MUS (Fig.…”
Section: Effects Of Gabaergic Drugsmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Their echolocation calls are characterized by the presence of a long constantfrequency component (Schnitzler, 1968;Neuweiler et al, 1987;Konstantinov et al, 1988). The frequency of calls emitted while the bat is perched is called the resting frequency (RF; Schnitzler, 1968).…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Horseshoe bats are insectivorous bats that hunt for insects in acoustically dense surroundings, scrub jungle and canopy (Neuweiler et al 1987). They encounter different types of interferences from reflections depending on the hunting strategy, i.e.…”
Section: Temporal and Spectral Gating Mechanisms And Clutter Rejectionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…The Sri Lankan rufous horseshoe bat, Rhinolophus rouxi, catches flying insects in the jungle on the wing or with a hang-and-wait strategy (Neuweiler et al 1987). While searching for prey the horseshoe bats emit long (20-60 ms) echolocation signals which consist of a pure tone component (CF) of an individual frequency called resting frequency (RF) within a species-specific frequency range of 72-79 kHz.…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…The Lombard effect has been used as a therapeutic tool to improve speech intelligibility in patients suffering from Parkinson's disease, and is also relevant to the study of phonetics and linguistics as well as in the context of animal behavior and the evolution of vocal plasticity (21). Therefore, studying the Lombard effect, the associated vocal changes, and their interaction between each other may lead to a better understanding of both the phenomenology and the underlying neurobiological control mechanisms (23).Echolocating bats are an excellent animal model to study the effects of masking ambient noise on vocal behavior; they are highly vocal and constantly adjust their echolocation pulses to optimize signal detection by monitoring the returning echo signals, which represent the feedback from their own voice (24,25). In addition, the bats' large hearing range of some 100 kHz (26) (Fig.…”
mentioning
confidence: 99%