1991
DOI: 10.1007/bf00172174
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Foraging in groups of water striders (Gerris remigis): effects of variability in prey arrivals and handling times

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
3
1
1

Citation Types

3
15
0

Year Published

1993
1993
2004
2004

Publication Types

Select...
8

Relationship

0
8

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 26 publications
(18 citation statements)
references
References 23 publications
3
15
0
Order By: Relevance
“…Dominant convict cichlids were able to defend the asynchronously arriving food effectively, and hence increased their share of the food. Such increases in monopolization have been previously observed as the arrival of food (Blanckenhorn 1991; Grant & Kramer 1992; Bryant & Grant 1995; Blanckenhorn et al. 1998) or mates (Bryant & Grant 1995; Grant et al.…”
Section: Discussionsupporting
confidence: 53%
See 1 more Smart Citation
“…Dominant convict cichlids were able to defend the asynchronously arriving food effectively, and hence increased their share of the food. Such increases in monopolization have been previously observed as the arrival of food (Blanckenhorn 1991; Grant & Kramer 1992; Bryant & Grant 1995; Blanckenhorn et al. 1998) or mates (Bryant & Grant 1995; Grant et al.…”
Section: Discussionsupporting
confidence: 53%
“…For example, resource monopolization might increase as resource arrival becomes more asynchronous, even in a non-aggressive species (Fig. 1b), because superior scramble competitors can potentially compete for each newly arriving resource unit (Blanckenhorn 1991;Grant & Kramer 1992;Grant et al 1995). However, resource monopolization will be higher when aggressive individuals are able to defend good sites and hence acquire even more of the asynchronous resource (Fig.…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…With the exception of a study by Grant and Kramer (1992), the few tests of the hypothesis focus only on measures of monopolization, primarily the variance of male mating success, and the results have either been equivocal (Gatz, 1981;Ridley, 1986, but see Altmann, 1990) or opposite to the predicted direction (Ryan, 1985). However, Blanckenhorn (1991) has recently shown that monopolization of food by water striders, Gerris remigis, decreased when temporal clumping increased, as predicted by the theory. Grant and Kramer (1992) tested the temporal clumping hypothesis using groups of six, individually tagged zebrafish, Brachydanio rerio, competing for 300 Daphnia pulex prey.…”
Section: Temporal Clumpingmentioning
confidence: 73%
“…Partial correlation analysis of the number of aggressive acts performed and fish size (L F ) with: (1) number of pellets captured, (2) number of times in the upstream-most position of a group, (3) number of focal positions used, (4) number of times alone, (5) size of the aggressive area, and (6) the upstream-most position within groups; a position that has been shown to produce higher feeding rates in other drift-feeding, stream taxa (Blanckenhorn, 1991b;Nakano, 1995). Similar to the present findings for rosyside dace, dominant individuals in stream-dwelling, drift-feeding fishes and water striders have been shown to place themselves upstream of conspecifics (Blanckenhorn, 1991b;Hughes, 1992;Nakano & Furukawa-Tanaka, 1994;Nakano, 1995;Katano, 1996). The study suggest that rosyside dace also utilized intraspecific aggression for defence of a particular position relative to neighbouring conspecifics rather than an exclusive-use foraging area (i.e.…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…The feeders dropped groups of pellets into the stream at the same time, and thus, increased temporal clumping of food arrival relative to field conditions (Freeman & Grossman, 1992). Such temporal clumping of prey can produce increased foraging rates for subordinates, because dominants may not be able to monopolize a large number of simultaneously arriving prey (Blanckenhorn, 1991b;Grant & Kramer, 1992). Occupying the upstream-most position in a foraging group can also be adaptive because it ensures the first opportunity to capture the scarce, though energetically important, large prey present in the drift, as suggested by Freeman & Grossman (1992).…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 99%