1999
DOI: 10.1080/135062899394948
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Formation of a Categorical Representation for the Spatial Relation Between by 6- to 7-month-old Infants

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
1
1
1

Citation Types

5
42
0

Year Published

2000
2000
2020
2020

Publication Types

Select...
4
4
2

Relationship

2
8

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 58 publications
(47 citation statements)
references
References 34 publications
5
42
0
Order By: Relevance
“…), so it must be driven purely by the learners' construal of the corresponding spatial scenes. Interestingly, just as in the present data, children's extension of containment terms is unlikely to be due to a conceptual inability to differentiate going-between from going-into events: There is evidence that even infants possess the concept BETWEEN (Quinn, Doran, & Papafragou, 2011;Quinn, Norris, Pasko, Schmader, & Mash, 1999). A more likely explanation is that children lack the more specific adposition between and co-opt containment expressions to express between-relations since the space between two objects can be treated as a bounded volume (see Johnston & Slobin, 1979;Skordos, Johanson, & Papafragou, unpublished data; for cross-linguistic evidence for the late emergence of between).…”
mentioning
confidence: 62%
“…), so it must be driven purely by the learners' construal of the corresponding spatial scenes. Interestingly, just as in the present data, children's extension of containment terms is unlikely to be due to a conceptual inability to differentiate going-between from going-into events: There is evidence that even infants possess the concept BETWEEN (Quinn, Doran, & Papafragou, 2011;Quinn, Norris, Pasko, Schmader, & Mash, 1999). A more likely explanation is that children lack the more specific adposition between and co-opt containment expressions to express between-relations since the space between two objects can be treated as a bounded volume (see Johnston & Slobin, 1979;Skordos, Johanson, & Papafragou, unpublished data; for cross-linguistic evidence for the late emergence of between).…”
mentioning
confidence: 62%
“…For example, in contrast to older infants (6-7 months), the representation of above versus below in younger infants (3-4 months) is specific to the particular objects in the above and below positions (Quinn et al, 1996;Quinn, Polly, Furer, Dobson, & Narter, 2002). Furthermore, whereas 6-to 7-month-olds appear to represent the between relation, 3-to 4-month-olds do not (Quinn, Norris, Pasko, Schmader, & Mash, 1999).…”
Section: Other Spatial-relational Informationmentioning
confidence: 92%
“…Quinn and colleagues (Quinn, Cummins, Kase, Martin, & Weissman, 1996;Quinn, Norris, Pasko, Schmader, & Mash, 1999), for instance, previously showed that the ability to form categories of static spatial relations develops at different times for different spatial relations. For example, infants can form a category of the location 'over' before they can form a category of the location 'between.…”
Section: Conceptual Foundations For Verb Learning 25mentioning
confidence: 99%