2022
DOI: 10.1016/j.cognition.2022.105027
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

From “haves” to “have nots”: Developmental declines in subjective social status reflect children's growing consideration of what they do not have

Help me understand this report
View preprint versions

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
1
1
1
1

Citation Types

5
27
1

Year Published

2022
2022
2025
2025

Publication Types

Select...
9

Relationship

2
7

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 18 publications
(33 citation statements)
references
References 74 publications
5
27
1
Order By: Relevance
“…We had predicted that the effect of SSS on risky decisions would emerge with age because children generally tend to report higher status when younger [ 44 , 45 ]. As children get older, SSS estimates also decline reflecting an explicit awareness of wealth status [ 46 ]. This pattern for SSS appeared in our data (median split: M younger = 8; M older = 6.65), but SSS remained above the midpoint of the scale for our age range.…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 99%
See 1 more Smart Citation
“…We had predicted that the effect of SSS on risky decisions would emerge with age because children generally tend to report higher status when younger [ 44 , 45 ]. As children get older, SSS estimates also decline reflecting an explicit awareness of wealth status [ 46 ]. This pattern for SSS appeared in our data (median split: M younger = 8; M older = 6.65), but SSS remained above the midpoint of the scale for our age range.…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Research with adults and adolescents has found that SSS and SES are distinct constructs that have additive effects on health outcomes [42,43]. For children, multiple studies have found younger children tend to rate themselves higher on SSS even when this does not reflect their actual SES [44][45][46]. By 10 years of age, children report SSS that is in line with SES, a change that is explained in part by children recognizing what they do not have as opposed to what they have [46,47].…”
Section: (A) Cognitive Development and Risky Decisionsmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Research on children's perceptions of their social status is scarce. There is evidence from interview studies (e.g., Heberle & Carter, 2020; Weinger, 1998) that children between 4 and 10 years are aware of their relative wealth or poverty, but children's ratings of their family socioeconomic status on measurement scales remain a vague ‘in the middle’ (Chen et al, 2019; Rauscher et al, 2017) until 10 years when their estimates begin to correlate with objective measures (Mistry et al, 2015; Peretz‐Lange et al, 2022).…”
Section: Subjective Social Statusmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…People start noticing inequalities between social groups early in life (Rhodes & Baron, 2019; Santhanagopalan et al, 2022; Shutts, 2015). For example, by at least 5 to 7 years of age, children are aware that White people tend to be wealthier than Black people (Elenbaas & Killen, 2016; Mandalaywala et al, 2020; Olson et al, 2012), that men tend to hold higher-status occupations than women (Liben et al, 2001; Miller et al, 2018), and that people of high socioeconomic status tend to have more desirable possessions (Dore et al, 2018; Peretz-Lange et al, 2022).…”
Section: Making Sense Of What Causes Inequalitymentioning
confidence: 99%