1998
DOI: 10.1016/s0926-6410(98)00004-4
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Functional MRI studies of spatial and nonspatial working memory

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
1
1
1

Citation Types

87
524
6
11

Year Published

2001
2001
2004
2004

Publication Types

Select...
6
2

Relationship

0
8

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 897 publications
(628 citation statements)
references
References 84 publications
87
524
6
11
Order By: Relevance
“…Yet, other areas including the dorsal premotor regions in the superior frontal sulcus and posterior parietal regions were active during the delay, congruent with arguably the most consistent finding among neuroimaging studies of working memory (e.g. [23,49,50]). The authors concluded that the DLPFC does not store active representations of the items; it selects the appropriate memory-guided response.…”
Section: Processes That Support Maintenance Selection Processessupporting
confidence: 76%
“…Yet, other areas including the dorsal premotor regions in the superior frontal sulcus and posterior parietal regions were active during the delay, congruent with arguably the most consistent finding among neuroimaging studies of working memory (e.g. [23,49,50]). The authors concluded that the DLPFC does not store active representations of the items; it selects the appropriate memory-guided response.…”
Section: Processes That Support Maintenance Selection Processessupporting
confidence: 76%
“…According to the model, ventrolateral prefrontal cortex (PFC; Brodmann's area [BA] 44, 45, and 47) is responsible for maintenance of information in the visuospatial and phonological stores, and dorsolateral PFC (BA 9 and 46) is required for CES processes of monitoring and manipulation (Petrides, 1994). This model is broadly supported by reviews of human imaging studies of working memory (D'Esposito et al, 1998;Owen, 1997). These studies contrast specific functions (e.g., maintenance vs. maintenance plus monitoring and manipulation) and generally find that ventrolateral PFC mediates maintenance of verbal and non-verbal information and that dorsolateral PFC is involved in executive components of monitoring and manipulation (Berman, Austin-Lane, Esposito, Van Horn, & Weinberger, 1996;Klingberg, O'Sullivan, & Roland, 1997;Manoach et al, 1997;Salmon et al, 1996;Stern et al, 2000;Tsukiura et al, 2001).…”
Section: Nih-pa Author Manuscriptmentioning
confidence: 83%
“…Support of domain specificity from the human neuroimaging literature has been inconsistent. Although several studies found prefrontal regions that were differentially activated by working memory for faces versus spatial locations (Courtney, Ungerleider, Keil, & Haxby, 1996;Ungerleider, Courtney, & Haxby, 1998), there was substantial functional overlap (Haxby, Petit, Ungerleider, & Courtney, 2000), and several recent reviews and meta-analyses failed to support domain specificity for dorsal versus ventral aspects of the PFC (D'Esposito et al, 1998;Owen, 1997).Because the fractal n-back task is a measure of object identification (what) rather than of spatial localization (where), the current study could not test domain specificity models of dorsal versus ventral PFC function. However, the current paradigm could test a variant of (Awh et al, 1996;Jonides et al, 1993;Smith et al, 1996).…”
mentioning
confidence: 99%
See 1 more Smart Citation
“…Alternatively, another model of prefrontal organization (DÕEsposito et al, 1998;Owen et al, 1998;Petrides, 1996;Postle, Berger, & DÕEsposito, 1999) suggests a subdivision of the prefrontal cortex depending rather upon the type of processing than on the informational domain. According to this model, monitoring and manipulation of information within working memory depend on the mid-dorsolateral frontal cortex, whereas maintenance of information involves the mid-ventrolateral and -dorsolateral frontal cortex (Postle et al, 1999).…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 99%