2018
DOI: 10.1016/j.lmot.2017.02.006
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Further evaluation of blocked trials to teach intraverbal responses under complex stimulus control: Effects of criterion-level probes

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
1
1

Citation Types

0
4
0
1

Year Published

2019
2019
2023
2023

Publication Types

Select...
6

Relationship

0
6

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 7 publications
(5 citation statements)
references
References 22 publications
0
4
0
1
Order By: Relevance
“…Various types of supplementary stimuli have been evaluated in the literature (verbal, textual, or pictorial), though findings from Coon and Miguel (2012) suggest that the relative efficacy of different prompts is a product of the participant's learning history. More recently, transfer of stimulus control procedures have been combined with blocked trials (e.g., Haggar, Ingvarsson, & Braun, 2018;Ingvarsson, Kramer, Carp, Pétursdóttir, & Macias, 2016) and differential observing responses (e.g., Kisamore, Karsten, & Mann, 2016) to establish complex intraverbals. These studies have been essential in developing effective procedures to directly teach intraverbals or, as Palmer (2016) recently clarified, "instances in which reinforcement of contiguous or correlated usage with a verbal antecedent has been observed or can plausibly be inferred" (pp.…”
mentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Various types of supplementary stimuli have been evaluated in the literature (verbal, textual, or pictorial), though findings from Coon and Miguel (2012) suggest that the relative efficacy of different prompts is a product of the participant's learning history. More recently, transfer of stimulus control procedures have been combined with blocked trials (e.g., Haggar, Ingvarsson, & Braun, 2018;Ingvarsson, Kramer, Carp, Pétursdóttir, & Macias, 2016) and differential observing responses (e.g., Kisamore, Karsten, & Mann, 2016) to establish complex intraverbals. These studies have been essential in developing effective procedures to directly teach intraverbals or, as Palmer (2016) recently clarified, "instances in which reinforcement of contiguous or correlated usage with a verbal antecedent has been observed or can plausibly be inferred" (pp.…”
mentioning
confidence: 99%
“…We need to determine a straightforward format to concisely present the skills present and deficit. It would be useful for readers to know the participants' history with a particular procedure or skill, such as the experiments that reported participants had previously been exposed to intraverbal training (e.g., Dickes & Kodak, 2015; Ingvarsson et al., 2016) or when participants had not yet mastered verbal conditional discriminations (e.g., Haggar et al., 2018). It would also be useful to know if the participant had mastered simple intraverbals and how many were in their repertoire.…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Twelve studies evaluated the effects of antecedent (e.g., repeating vs. not repeating the discriminative stimulus at each prompting step; Humphreys, Polick, Howk, Thaxton, & Ivancic, 2013) or consequence (e.g., token reinforcement vs. natural contingencies; Mason, Davis, & Andrews, 2015) manipulations during intraverbal training, as well as the outcomes of different instructional formats in the acquisition of intraverbal responses (e.g., blocked trials; Haggar, Ingvarsson, & Braun, 2017). Haq et al (2015) compared intraverbal acquisition of massed and distributed trials in three children with ASD.…”
Section: Intraverbalsmentioning
confidence: 99%