2004
DOI: 10.1016/j.pain.2003.10.014
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Further results about pain rating by patients and physicians: reply to Chibnall and Tait

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
2
1
1

Citation Types

0
4
0

Year Published

2007
2007
2011
2011

Publication Types

Select...
3

Relationship

1
2

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 3 publications
(4 citation statements)
references
References 0 publications
0
4
0
Order By: Relevance
“…These results cohere with previous studies that showed that, both for chronic and acute pain, the higher the patient's rating, the greater the observers' disbelief. 5,12,39,40 However, it should be noted that this effect was really very small, being the Cohen's d of 0.12, which is not even a small effect according to Cohen's benchmark 50 of 0.2. The statistical significance of this effect using a weak manipulation (e.g., text-based vignettes) suggests that it is worth examining how strong the effect might be with more naturalistic paradigms.…”
Section: The Influence Of the Level Of The Patient's Rating On The Extent Of Anchoringmentioning
confidence: 89%
See 1 more Smart Citation
“…These results cohere with previous studies that showed that, both for chronic and acute pain, the higher the patient's rating, the greater the observers' disbelief. 5,12,39,40 However, it should be noted that this effect was really very small, being the Cohen's d of 0.12, which is not even a small effect according to Cohen's benchmark 50 of 0.2. The statistical significance of this effect using a weak manipulation (e.g., text-based vignettes) suggests that it is worth examining how strong the effect might be with more naturalistic paradigms.…”
Section: The Influence Of the Level Of The Patient's Rating On The Extent Of Anchoringmentioning
confidence: 89%
“…First, we hypothesized that the observers would exhibit a tendency to anchor to their initial impressions of pain judgment (Hypothesis 1). Secondly, based on previous studies that showed that both for chronic and acute pain, the higher the patient's rating, the greater the observers' discrepancy, 5,12,39,40 we tested the hypothesis of a different extent of anchoring depending on the patient's self-report. Specifically, we expected a greater extent of anchoring for the cases in which patients' ratings were higher than the participants' initial impression compared with those in which the patients' ratings were lower than the participants' initial impression (Hypothesis 2).…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Although the usual methods for estimating pain, such as numerical rating scale [1] or a visual analogue scale [2], have been validated, the clinical impact of their use is uncertain. Clinicians continue in many cases to be insufficiently aware of patients' pain [3][4][5][6][7] and to rate it at lower levels than do patients themselves, for both acute [8][9][10][11][12] and chronic [13] pain. When asked why they give lower ratings, clinicians explain that they are trained to look for behavioral signs of pain and that, accordingly, when they judge the behavioral evidence or the nature of the illness as ''discordant'' with the patients' stated level of pain, they will, consciously or unconsciously, adjust their own ratings in light of this conflicting information.…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Yet they provided an insightful basis for subsequent studies. Furthermore, Marquié et al (2004) didn't find any difference in the degree of miscalibration between novices and experts. 1 The latter factor was introduced on the basis of a preliminary qualitative study in which it emerged that healthcare professionals were influenced by the patients' place of origin (Montali et al, 2009).…”
Section: Participantsmentioning
confidence: 87%