2020
DOI: 10.1016/j.cognition.2020.104390
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Future thinking about social targets: The influence of prediction outcome on memory

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
5

Citation Types

6
28
1

Year Published

2020
2020
2024
2024

Publication Types

Select...
10

Relationship

8
2

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 26 publications
(35 citation statements)
references
References 59 publications
6
28
1
Order By: Relevance
“…In the present research, students displayed less gender bias favoring men in cognitive accessibility and accurate memory concerning a campus leadership role when it was called head compared to the masculine generic term master . While gender-related expectations and stereotypes can make gender incongruent information more accessible and produce relatively better memory for it under particular circumstances—when people notice a salient discrepancy, are motivated to resolve it, and have adequate processing resources to do so (Fiske & Neuberg, 1990; Hess, 2005)—our findings are consistent with a large body of evidence indicating that congruent information is typically more accessible and can be better remembered (Bayen et al, 2000; Frankenstein et al, 2020). This advantage for congruent information has been shown for memory of faces during impression formation (Cassidy et al, 2012; Eberhardt et al, 2003; Nash et al, 2010) as well as specifically for gender stereotypical information (Ellemers, 2018).…”
Section: Discussionsupporting
confidence: 84%
“…In the present research, students displayed less gender bias favoring men in cognitive accessibility and accurate memory concerning a campus leadership role when it was called head compared to the masculine generic term master . While gender-related expectations and stereotypes can make gender incongruent information more accessible and produce relatively better memory for it under particular circumstances—when people notice a salient discrepancy, are motivated to resolve it, and have adequate processing resources to do so (Fiske & Neuberg, 1990; Hess, 2005)—our findings are consistent with a large body of evidence indicating that congruent information is typically more accessible and can be better remembered (Bayen et al, 2000; Frankenstein et al, 2020). This advantage for congruent information has been shown for memory of faces during impression formation (Cassidy et al, 2012; Eberhardt et al, 2003; Nash et al, 2010) as well as specifically for gender stereotypical information (Ellemers, 2018).…”
Section: Discussionsupporting
confidence: 84%
“…Indeed, work in other domains suggests that memory for information about future events tend to be better remembered than information that does not involve future planning (Klein et al, 2010). This in turn is consistent with other work suggesting that people use the contents of memory in order to think about the future (Szpunar et al, 2007;Szpunar, 2010;Frankenstein et al, 2020). In the current study, it may be that better memory for highly plausible items, such as a red apple, may be useful for future planning, such as deciding when food is ready to be consumed.…”
Section: Discussionsupporting
confidence: 83%
“…Taken as a whole, the research on generation constraint and context memory suggests that constraint likely influences memory for some contextual details more than others, further highlighting the need for more research to identify the boundary conditions of generation constraint effects. Understanding how to improve memory is an important scientific goal (Frankenstein et al, 2020;Leach, McCurdy, Trumbo, Matzen, & Leshikar, 2018;Leshikar, Duarte, & Hertzog, 2012;Leshikar et al, 2017;Matzen, Trumbo, Leach, & Leshikar, 2015;Meyers, McCurdy, Leach, Thomas, & Leshikar, 2020), and this work adds to that endeavor.…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 99%