Recently, Carroll and Nelson (1993) presented research suggesting that general-information questions might represent a boundary condition for the generation effect. The present research focused on whether the generation effect did, in fact, generalize to such questions. In Experiment 1,when subjects read or generated the answers to general-information questions, a generation advantage was demonstrated on a 47-h delayed cued-recall test. However, when the Carroll and Nelson procedure was mimicked by requiring subjects to make an initial attempt to answer the questions, the generation advantage was reduced such that it was no longer statistically significant. In Experiments 2 and 3, the findings ofthe first experiment generalized to a free-recall test. Thus, general-information questions do not represent a boundary condition for the generation effect.The generation effect occurs when an individual better remembers information that he or she produces than information provided by an external source (Jacoby, 1978;Slamecka & Graf, 1978). The generation effect has proven to be remarkably robust across a number of different paradigms and types of materials (see, e.g., Begg & Snider's, 1987, review of the empirical findings). Recently, however, the generality of the effect was challenged. Specifically, Carroll and Nelson (1993) cast doubt on one's ability to extend the laboratory findings to more naturalistic learning situations.Carroll and Nelson (1993) presented subjects with general-information questions (e.g., Which planet was the last to be discovered?). A subject's task was to answer each question out loud. Ifthe subject knew the answer, the question was discarded and another question was shown. If the subject did not know the answer, one of two procedures was followed. In the read condition, the subject was shown the question again with the answer written on a card. The subject had 15sec to study the question and the corresponding answer. In the generate condition, the subject was shown the question and clues to the answer. Either the clues were the number ofletters in the word followed by the first letter, then the second letter, and so on until the subject correctly generated the answer (Experiment 1), or the clue was an anagram ofthe answer (Experiments 2-7). The recall test occurred 1 week after study (Experiments 1-4, 6, and 7) or after a 10-min distractor task (Experiment 5). Furthermore, the generation task was made either easier or This research was supported by a grant from the Keck Foundation. I would like to thank William Friedman, Marie Carroll, Michael Pressley, Norman Slamecka, John Gardiner, Henry Roediger, and anonymous reviewers for their thoughtful comments on a previous draft. I also thank Amy Hughes for her assistance in running subjects and Gudrun Jonsdottir for scoring subjects' recall. Correspondence concerning this article should be addressed to P.A. deWinstanley, Department of Psychology, Oberlin College, Oberlin, OH 44074-1086 (e-mail: patty@occs.cs.oberlin. edu). more difficult by the pr...