2019
DOI: 10.1016/j.neuron.2019.03.034
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Grid-like Neural Representations Support Olfactory Navigation of a Two-Dimensional Odor Space

Abstract: Highlights d How the human brain supports navigation in an odorous landscape is poorly understood d Subjects learn to orient within a 2D intensity space defined by two different odors d Odor navigation elicits grid-cell-like activity in prefrontal and entorhinal cortices d Findings suggest a mechanism by which the brain constructs olfactory cognitive maps

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
2
1
1

Citation Types

13
163
1

Year Published

2019
2019
2023
2023

Publication Types

Select...
3
2
2

Relationship

0
7

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 151 publications
(177 citation statements)
references
References 54 publications
13
163
1
Order By: Relevance
“…While past studies have identified neural signals in the HC and EC indicative of a cognitive map primarily using continuous task dimensions with online sensory feedback during task performance (e.g. visual, auditory, vestibular) (Aronov, Nevers, & Tank, 2017;Bao et al, 2019;Constantinescu et al, 2016;Doeller, Barry, & Burgess, 2010;Howard Eichenbaum & Cohen, 2014;Hafting et al, 2005;Nau, Navarro Schröder, Bellmund, & Doeller, 2018;O'Keefe & Nadel, 1978;Theves, Fernandez, & Doeller, 2019), many important everyday decisions involve discrete entities that vary along multiple abstract dimensions that are sampled piecemeal, one experience at a time, in the absence of continuous sensory feedback, such as with whom to collaborate or where to eat. How the brain constructs a cognitive map of abstract relationships between discrete entities from piecemeal experiences is unclear.…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…While past studies have identified neural signals in the HC and EC indicative of a cognitive map primarily using continuous task dimensions with online sensory feedback during task performance (e.g. visual, auditory, vestibular) (Aronov, Nevers, & Tank, 2017;Bao et al, 2019;Constantinescu et al, 2016;Doeller, Barry, & Burgess, 2010;Howard Eichenbaum & Cohen, 2014;Hafting et al, 2005;Nau, Navarro Schröder, Bellmund, & Doeller, 2018;O'Keefe & Nadel, 1978;Theves, Fernandez, & Doeller, 2019), many important everyday decisions involve discrete entities that vary along multiple abstract dimensions that are sampled piecemeal, one experience at a time, in the absence of continuous sensory feedback, such as with whom to collaborate or where to eat. How the brain constructs a cognitive map of abstract relationships between discrete entities from piecemeal experiences is unclear.…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…This activity pattern resembles the patterns of transition matrix eigenvectors of hexagonal graphs 33,34 . Further, grid-like representations emerge in enviroments/tasks that are not spatial but share similar statistical structure [35][36][37] . These observations may suggest that grid cells can be used as basis functions for all environments in which the associations between the states are governed by the rules of 2D Euclidean space.…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…We further assumed, for simplicity, that the two dimensions of the hexagonal grid are equal and the number of nodes in a community is also equal, hence θ defines a vector of possible number of nodes in a graph. For hexagonal graphs we considred N = [25,36,49], for a graph with underlying community structure N = [28,35,42] with equal prior probability. We emphasis that the basis set ( ) for each transition structure within a structural form is a scaled or trancated version of a general basis set for that structural form.…”
Section: Inferring Graph Sizementioning
confidence: 99%
“…Grid cells, typically recorded from freely navigating rats and humans (Hafting, Fyhn, Molden, Moser, & Moser, ; Sargolini et al, ; Jacobs et al, ), display a regularly spaced pattern of neural firing (“grids”) that span the environment. In addition to their presence during spatial navigation, studies have also observed such grid coding during tasks involving representation of conceptual categories, imagination, and scene processing (Bao et al, ; Bellmund, Deuker, Schroder, & Doeller, ; Constantinescu, O'reilly, & Behrens, ; Garvert, Dolan, & Behrens, ; Horner, Bisby, Zotow, Bush, & Burgess, ; Killian, Jutras, & Buffalo, ). One theoretical interpretation of such grid coding is that it forms the basis of a universal spatial metric representation that underlies many, if not all, forms of spatial and nonspatial representations (Behrens et al, ; Bellmund, Gardenfors, Moser, & Doeller, ; Bush, Barry, Manson, & Burgess, ; Hasselmo, Giocomo, Brandon, & Yoshida, ; Hawkins, Lewis, Klukas, Purdy, & Ahmad, ).…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Grid cells, typically recorded from freely navigating rats and humans (Hafting, Fyhn, Molden, Moser, & Moser, 2005;Sargolini et al, 2006;Jacobs et al, 2013), display a regularly spaced pattern of neural firing ("grids") that span the environment. In addition to their presence during spatial navigation, studies have also observed such grid coding during tasks involving representation of conceptual categories, imagination, and scene processing (Bao et al, 2019;Bellmund, Deuker, Schroder, & Doeller, 2016;Constantinescu, O'reilly, & Behrens, 2016;Garvert, Dolan, & Behrens, 2017;Horner, Bisby, Zotow, Bush, & Burgess, 2016;Killian, Jutras, & Buffalo, 2012).…”
mentioning
confidence: 99%