2018
DOI: 10.1016/j.jesp.2018.04.005
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Group-based biases influence learning about individual trustworthiness

Abstract: People often have generalised expectations of trustworthiness about ingroup and outgroup members, based on previous direct and indirect experience with these groups. How do these prior biases interact with new experiences when learning about individual group members' trustworthiness? These three studies are the first to examine the effect of group-level biases on learning about individuals' trustworthiness. Participants from the Netherlands and the United Kingdom played iterated Trust Games with trustworthy an… Show more

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
1
1
1

Citation Types

4
17
0

Year Published

2019
2019
2022
2022

Publication Types

Select...
7

Relationship

1
6

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 13 publications
(21 citation statements)
references
References 52 publications
4
17
0
Order By: Relevance
“…People tend to adhere to ingroup biases to maintain positive social identities . Ingroup bias has been extensively documented (Vermue, Seger, & Sanfey, 2018), but recent research indicates that contextual cues can alter ingroup self-categorization (Fasoli, Cadinu, Carnaghi, Galdi, Guizzo, & Tassara, 2018) and determine whether individuals think, feel, and act in alignment with ingroups (us) or outgroups (them) (Turner, Brown, & Tajfel, 1979;Turner, Hogg, Oakes, Reicher, & Wetherell, 1987). Context dynamically indicates ingroup/outgroup identity cues and can thus activate understandings and actions in terms of norms, values, and behaviors relevant to particular identities (Aaker & Akutsu, 2009;Kirmani, 2009;Oyserman, 2009Oyserman, , 2013Shavitt, Torelli, & Wong, 2009).…”
Section: Ingroup Bias Identity Cues and Ethical Consumptionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…People tend to adhere to ingroup biases to maintain positive social identities . Ingroup bias has been extensively documented (Vermue, Seger, & Sanfey, 2018), but recent research indicates that contextual cues can alter ingroup self-categorization (Fasoli, Cadinu, Carnaghi, Galdi, Guizzo, & Tassara, 2018) and determine whether individuals think, feel, and act in alignment with ingroups (us) or outgroups (them) (Turner, Brown, & Tajfel, 1979;Turner, Hogg, Oakes, Reicher, & Wetherell, 1987). Context dynamically indicates ingroup/outgroup identity cues and can thus activate understandings and actions in terms of norms, values, and behaviors relevant to particular identities (Aaker & Akutsu, 2009;Kirmani, 2009;Oyserman, 2009Oyserman, , 2013Shavitt, Torelli, & Wong, 2009).…”
Section: Ingroup Bias Identity Cues and Ethical Consumptionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…The sample size was based on power calculations using the coefficient and standard error of previous studies using the same paradigm but a withinsubject design of group, using the same MatLab script asVermue et al (2018). In these previous studies, the effect size of the interaction of interest and a sample size of N = 48 resulted in an obtained power of 0.76.…”
mentioning
confidence: 99%
“…One important manifestation of ingroup favoritism is the tendency to trust fellow ingroup members more than outgroup members (Balliet, Wu, & De Dreu, 2014; Dovidio & Gaertner, 2010; Fiske, 1998; Insko et al, 1998; Vermue, Seger, & Sanfey, 2018). The intergroup relations literature provides abundant evidence of presumptive, exaggerated trust toward those categorized as ingroup members (Dovidio & Gaertner, 2010; Hewstone et al, 2002; Kramer, 1999).…”
Section: Theory and Hypothesesmentioning
confidence: 99%