1983
DOI: 10.1007/bf00289676
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Group sex composition and member task motivation

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
2
1
1

Citation Types

3
23
1

Year Published

1984
1984
2010
2010

Publication Types

Select...
8

Relationship

3
5

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 27 publications
(27 citation statements)
references
References 36 publications
3
23
1
Order By: Relevance
“…The behavior of females in two studies (Jackson, Williams, & Latane, 1978;Swanson & Tjosvold, 1979) was consistent with the prescriptions of traditional sex roles; females were less productive when they worked with a male partner than with a female partner. However, the validity of these findings is clouded by several methodological prob1ems.l On the other hand, Kerr and Sullaway (1983) found that both male and female members of mixed-sex dyads outperformed isolated individuals, whereas members of same-sex dyads performed no better than individuals.…”
Section: Please Scroll Down For Articlementioning
confidence: 95%
See 1 more Smart Citation
“…The behavior of females in two studies (Jackson, Williams, & Latane, 1978;Swanson & Tjosvold, 1979) was consistent with the prescriptions of traditional sex roles; females were less productive when they worked with a male partner than with a female partner. However, the validity of these findings is clouded by several methodological prob1ems.l On the other hand, Kerr and Sullaway (1983) found that both male and female members of mixed-sex dyads outperformed isolated individuals, whereas members of same-sex dyads performed no better than individuals.…”
Section: Please Scroll Down For Articlementioning
confidence: 95%
“…Several other models were advanced by Kerr and Sullaway (1983) as possible explanations for their findings (viz., both sexes worked hardest in mixedsex dyads). One is an esteem maintenance model, which suggests that in a cooperative work setting both sexes are much more concerned with demonstrating their value to the group and their competence to an opposite sex partner than to a same-sex partner.…”
Section: Please Scroll Down For Articlementioning
confidence: 97%
“…Empirical demonstrations of the opposite result, where one or more workers actually try harder when performing in a group or team than as individuals have been far less frequent and prominent. Yet there is some evidence of such group motivation gains in the literature, particularly in the last decade or so (e.g., Erev, Bornstein, & Galili, 1993;Hertel, Kerr, & Messé, 2000a;Kerr & MacCoun, 1984;Kerr & Sullaway, 1983;Stroebe, Diehl, & Abakoumkin, 1996;Williams & Karau, 1991;Worchel, Rothgerber, Day, Hart, & Butemeyer, 1998). The present study focuses on one such group motivation-gain phenomenon, the Köhler effect (Köhler, 1926)-a tendency of less able workers to perform better when members of a team working under conjunctive task demands (where the performance of the least capable member defines the group's level of performance; Steiner, 1972) than when working individually.…”
mentioning
confidence: 95%
“…Additionally, the likelihood of observing motivation gains can be influenced by information about the comparison target. Intergroup settings that allow for comparisons between groups, or between individuals who differ on a salient social category (e.g., gender) have not only been shown to eliminate social loafing (e.g., Erev, Bornstein, & Galili, 1993;Harkins & Szymanski, 1989;James & Greenberg, 1989;Mulvey & Ribbens, 1999;Ouwerkerk et al, 2000;Worchel et al, 1998) but to elicit motivation gains as well (e.g., Kerr & Sullaway, 1983;Lount, Messé, & Kerr, 2000;Lount & Phillips, 2007).…”
Section: Motivation Gains In Intergroup Settingsmentioning
confidence: 98%