2021
DOI: 10.1080/1357650x.2021.1990313
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Hand preference for unimanual and bimanual tasks: Evidence from questionnaires and preferential reaching

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
1
1

Citation Types

0
1
0

Year Published

2022
2022
2024
2024

Publication Types

Select...
4

Relationship

0
4

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 4 publications
(2 citation statements)
references
References 38 publications
0
1
0
Order By: Relevance
“…These authors reported that the dominant arm had a higher choice rate than the non-dominant arm, which was related to a smaller error at the endpoint and a linear reach trajectory. Furthermore, some studies used a task that combined reaching and grasping and/or pouring water into a cup [19][20][21][22] . The studies of the grasping and pouring movements suggest that factors such as cup handle orientation are related to arm choice, independent of the cost of the reach.…”
Section: Brydenmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…These authors reported that the dominant arm had a higher choice rate than the non-dominant arm, which was related to a smaller error at the endpoint and a linear reach trajectory. Furthermore, some studies used a task that combined reaching and grasping and/or pouring water into a cup [19][20][21][22] . The studies of the grasping and pouring movements suggest that factors such as cup handle orientation are related to arm choice, independent of the cost of the reach.…”
Section: Brydenmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…However, the current results also suggested that the impact of providing upper extremity assistance may vary across tasks. Similarly, previous research showed that displayed hand preferences might depend on nature of the task (e.g., unimanual vs. bimanual [104]), skill level required (e.g., skilled vs. unskilled tasks, complex vs. easy tasks [105][106][107]), location of the presented object (e.g., left-space vs. right-space, midline position vs. contralateral space [72,[104][105][106]108]), the number of object presentations (e.g., 9 vs. 32 [109], and the object type (e.g., tool vs. dowel [105]).…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 57%