1970
DOI: 10.2466/pms.1970.30.2.343
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Handedness: Proficiency versus Stated Preference

Abstract: However, preferred hand performance was superior on a1-most al1 tasks. It was suggested that preferred hand performance is characterized by "Automatization" of the skills involved in hand performance. The results cast serious doubt on the ~lidity of using questionnaires of hand preferences to measure the degree of established handedness.

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
1
1
1

Citation Types

2
34
0
2

Year Published

1974
1974
2023
2023

Publication Types

Select...
6
2

Relationship

0
8

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 122 publications
(38 citation statements)
references
References 21 publications
2
34
0
2
Order By: Relevance
“…The mean times to make the 40 taps were 6.96 sec for males and 7.55 sec for females [t(22) = 1.95, p < .10]. Males have been reported to be significantly faster than females in tapping rate where precision is not important (e.g., Barnsley & Rabinowitz, 1970). Responses in the original experiment, as well as for the supplementary data, did not require precise movements.…”
Section: Resultsmentioning
confidence: 86%
“…The mean times to make the 40 taps were 6.96 sec for males and 7.55 sec for females [t(22) = 1.95, p < .10]. Males have been reported to be significantly faster than females in tapping rate where precision is not important (e.g., Barnsley & Rabinowitz, 1970). Responses in the original experiment, as well as for the supplementary data, did not require precise movements.…”
Section: Resultsmentioning
confidence: 86%
“…Our results are supported by some previous studies and are in contrast to others, which may be a reflection of the different tests of motor bias measuring different characteristics of bias. In humans, motor proficiency can be described using a wide range of unimanual tasks distributed over 10 distinct factors (Barnsley and Rabinovitch, 1970). Therefore the use of different tests of motor bias will result in different skills being measured, and thus different conclusions about motor bias being made.…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Right-left differences in performance have been studied on several tasks such as peg moving (Annett, 1970), fast (Peters & Durding, 1978) or regular (Wolff, 1977) finger tapping, dot filling (Tapley & Bryden, 1985), or the Purdue pegboard test (Barnsley & Rabinovitch, 1970;Fleishman & Hempel, 1954). Several studies have combined hand preference and right-left differences in performance to assess handedness (e.g., Bishop, 2001;Corey, Hurley, & Foundas, 2001;Peters & Durding, 1978).…”
mentioning
confidence: 99%
“…We devised a single laterality index based on (a) preference tests that assessed the spontaneous use of one or the other hand on several tasks, including writing and the spontaneous use of one eye to look through a hole; and (b) the right-left performance difference tested on the peg-moving task and on the unimanual crank-rotation task. We used two tasks because, hand efficiency being a multidimensional trait (Barnsley & Rabinovitch, 1970;Fleishman & Ellison, 1962;Rigal, 1992), it is clear to many researchers that using a single test of hand efficiency is not sufficient. The peg-moving task and the unimanual crank-rotation task differ on important skill traits such as distal versus proximal (the crank-rotation task is more proximal than the peg-moving task), learned versus less automatized (the crank-rotation task is a newer task than picking up pegs), and intermodal versus unimodal (eye-hand coordination for the peg-moving task vs. purely manual for the unimanual crank-rotation task).…”
mentioning
confidence: 99%