2012
DOI: 10.1017/s136898001200064x
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Health labelling can influence taste perception and use of table salt for reduced-sodium products

Abstract: Objective: To investigate the effect of front-of-pack labels on taste perception and use of table salt for currently available and sodium-reduced soups. Design: Within-subject design. Setting: Sensory laboratory. Subjects: Participants (n 50, mean age 34?8 (SD 13?6) years) were randomly served nine soups (250 ml each) across 3 d. Servings differed in: (i) health label (i.e. no health label, reduced-salt label or Heart Foundation Tick); and (ii) sodium reduction (no reduction -benchmark, 15 % less sodium or 30 … Show more

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
3
1
1

Citation Types

5
68
1
3

Year Published

2014
2014
2022
2022

Publication Types

Select...
9
1

Relationship

0
10

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 77 publications
(77 citation statements)
references
References 27 publications
5
68
1
3
Order By: Relevance
“…Expectations, generated by information provided, are reported to have an effect on hedonic ratings in the case of health and nutrition claims for biscuits (Carillo, Varela & Fiszman, 2012a;Carillo et al, 2012b), soup with a reduced salt health label (Liem et al, 2012a), and nutritional facts on milk and soybean beverages (Villegas, Carbonell & Costell, 2008). Expectations had no effect in two cases, one where nutritional facts were provided on yogurt and fermented milk (Bayarri, Carbonell, Barrios & Costell, 2010) and one where consumers were informed about alcohol-free wine (Meillon et al, 2010).…”
Section: Effects Of Expectations Generated By Health-related Credencementioning
confidence: 99%
“…Expectations, generated by information provided, are reported to have an effect on hedonic ratings in the case of health and nutrition claims for biscuits (Carillo, Varela & Fiszman, 2012a;Carillo et al, 2012b), soup with a reduced salt health label (Liem et al, 2012a), and nutritional facts on milk and soybean beverages (Villegas, Carbonell & Costell, 2008). Expectations had no effect in two cases, one where nutritional facts were provided on yogurt and fermented milk (Bayarri, Carbonell, Barrios & Costell, 2010) and one where consumers were informed about alcohol-free wine (Meillon et al, 2010).…”
Section: Effects Of Expectations Generated By Health-related Credencementioning
confidence: 99%
“…These comments came in equal proportions from the control group as the intervention group, indicating that store employees perceived a difference because they were expecting the bread to be different or due to attention bias. There is evidence that labelling foods as no/low/reduced salt can influence expectation and taste perception [24,25]. Liem et al (2012) found that when soups were labelled “now with reduced salt”, participants expected to like them less than the same soups without the labelling [25].…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Less educated respondents are less likely to check nutrition labels as an aid to lower salt intake (23, 24). To increase the value of food labels, there is need for a system that makes the salt content of foods easily apparent regardless of consumers’ ability to read (25). A scheme that includes color-coded interpretation of nutrient levels is most likely to help consumers identify at a glance those foods low in salt (26).…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 99%