Symmetry detection is slow when patterns are distorted by perspective, perhaps due to a time-consuming normalization process, or because discrimination relies on remaining weaker regularities in the retinal image. Participants viewed symmetrical or random dot patterns, either in a frontoparallel or slanted plane (±50°). One group performed a color discrimination task, while another performed a regularity discrimination task. We measured a symmetry-related eventrelated potential (ERP), beginning around 300 ms. During color discrimination, the ERP was reduced for slanted patterns, indexing only the remaining retinal structure. During regularity discrimination, the same ERP was view invariant, and identical for frontoparallel or slanted presentation. We conclude that normalization occurs rapidly during active symmetry discrimination, while symmetry-sensitive networks respond only to regularity in the retinal image when people are attending to other features.Descriptors: Symmetry, Event-related potentials, Sustained posterior negativity, View invariance, Perspective distortionThe two-dimensional retinal projection of an object changes dramatically as the observer adopts different vantage points. This produces novel inputs to the recognition system, but objects are nevertheless identified reliably and rapidly, and this formidable computational feat occurs unconsciously. Logothetis and Sheinberg (1996) concluded that some neurons are view invariant (firing to their preferred stimulus independent of view angle), some are view selective (firing more for some view angles), and that view invariance is more common for familiar objects than novel objects. It is also known that the neural response to faces becomes increasingly view invariant in higher visual regions (Axelrod & Yovel, 2012). Here, we measured whether the neural response to abstract visual symmetry is view invariant or view selective.Many visual systems are highly sensitive to symmetry, perhaps because it helps to identify objects against a background (Machilsen, Pauwels, & Wagemans, 2009), to achieve shape constancy (Pizlo & Stevenson, 1999), or because it indicates reproductive fitness (Tyler, 1995). Bilateral symmetry perception has been demonstrated in insects (Plowright, Evans, Leung, & Collin, 2011), birds (Møller, 1992), and humans (Julesz, 1971; Mach, 1886 Mach, / 1959. In humans, symmetry perception interacts with other figural cues (Bertamini, 2010;Treder & van der Helm, 2007), while models have been developed that extract symmetry from spatial filters (Dakin & Hess, 1997).Most psychophysical and neuroimaging researchers have used symmetric patterns in the frontoparallel plane, which produce a symmetrical retinal projection (e.g., Bertamini, Friedenberg, & Kubovy, 1997;Jacobsen & Höfel, 2003;Royer, 1981;Sasaki, Vanduffel, Knutsen, Tyler, & Tootell, 2005;Wenderoth, 1994). However, the benefits of symmetry perception presuppose the ability to recognize symmetrical objects from multiple viewpoints: After all, an observer will almost never enco...