1989
DOI: 10.1075/cilt.6
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Historical and Comparative Linguistics

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
1
1
1

Citation Types

2
49
0
2

Year Published

2002
2002
2013
2013

Publication Types

Select...
5
3

Relationship

0
8

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 431 publications
(53 citation statements)
references
References 0 publications
2
49
0
2
Order By: Relevance
“…The listener prefers that the child produce salient and distinct sound shapes to code distinct concepts, a preference that historical linguists have assumed to apply in adult language as well (e.g. Vennemann 1978, Anttila 1989. This emphasis on the communicative function of language brings us to another definition of phonology, one that supports the view that vocabulary acquisition drives phonological development.…”
Section: Vocabulary Acquisition and Phonological Developmentmentioning
confidence: 65%
“…The listener prefers that the child produce salient and distinct sound shapes to code distinct concepts, a preference that historical linguists have assumed to apply in adult language as well (e.g. Vennemann 1978, Anttila 1989. This emphasis on the communicative function of language brings us to another definition of phonology, one that supports the view that vocabulary acquisition drives phonological development.…”
Section: Vocabulary Acquisition and Phonological Developmentmentioning
confidence: 65%
“…The phonology involved in this word formation is similar to the two cases studied in the previous sections in that various nonconcatenative base modificational strategies are attested depending on the base form. This phase alternation is comprehensively described by Churchward (1940) and theoretically studied quite extensively in the earlier literature (Haudricourt 1958ab;Biggs 1959Biggs , 1965Milner 1971;Cairns 1976;Saito 1981;van der Hulst 1983;Janda 1984;McCarthy 1986bMcCarthy , 1989McCarthy , 2000cMester 1986;Besnier 1987;Hoeksema andJanda 1988, Odden 1988;Anttila 1989;Weeda 1992;Blevins 1994). All content words exhibit the phase alternation.…”
Section: The Incomplete Phase In Rotumanmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Examples of final segment deletion include Danish imperatives (Anderson 1975;Jones and Gade 1981;Allan, Holmes and Lundskaer-Nielsen 1995), the Rotuman incomplete phase (Churchward 1940;Haudricourt 1958ab;Biggs 1959Biggs , 1965Milner 1971;Cairns 1976;Saito 1981;van der Hulst 1983;Janda 1984;McCarthy 1986bMcCarthy , 1989McCarthy , 2000cMester 1986;Besnier 1987;Hoeksema and Janda 1988;Odden 1988;Anttila 1989;Weeda 1992;Blevins 1994), Icelandic deverbal nouns (Ore nik 1972Arnason 1980;Kiparsky 1984;Itô 1986;Benua 1995), Lardil nominatives (Hale 1973;Itô 1986;Wilkinson 1988;Weeda 1992;Prince and Smolensky 1993;Blevins 1997;Horwood 1999;Kurisu 1999), Tiberian Hebrew imperatives (Prince 1975;Benua 1995Benua , 1997, and Tiberian Hebrew jussives (Prince 1975;McCarthy 1979;Benua 1995Benua , 1997. Some examples of languages with final syllable deletion are Papago perfectives …”
mentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Unmistakable blends as in (1)-(3) above present direct evidence of a blending mechanism, but they appear to be relatively rare, particularly as a source of change. It is telling that most historical linguistics textbooks mention blending only in connection to contaminations in word formation (often as a source of irregularities in sound change) (Jeffers & Lehiste 1979;Anttila 1989;Aitchison 1991;Lehmann 1992;McMahon 1994;Trask 1996;Sihler 1999;Crowley 2010), while some textbooks fail to mention blending altogether (Bynon 1977;Hale 2007),…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Unmistakable blends as in (1)-(3) above present direct evidence of a blending mechanism, but they appear to be relatively rare, particularly as a source of change. It is telling that most historical linguistics textbooks mention blending only in connection to contaminations in word formation (often as a source of irregularities in sound change) (Jeffers & Lehiste 1979;Anttila 1989;Aitchison 1991; Lehmann 1992; McMahon 1994; Trask 1996; Sihler 1999;Crowley 2010), while some textbooks fail to mention blending altogether (Bynon 1977;Hale 2007),and only a few discuss blending in relation to syntactic change (Hock 1986;Croft 2000;Campbell 2004). It is further revealing that the changes caused by blending are described as "oddities" (Aitchison 1991: 177), "peripherally important" (Anttila 1989: 142) or "sporadic" (McMahon 1994: 75).…”
mentioning
confidence: 99%