2015
DOI: 10.1007/s00221-015-4321-x
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Hitting moving targets with a continuously changing temporal window

Abstract: Hitting a moving target requires that we do not miss the target when it is around the aimed position. The time available for us not to miss the target when it is at the position of interest is usually called the time window and depends on target's speed and size. These variables, among others, have been manipulated in previous studies but kept constant within the same trial or session. Here, we present results of a hitting task in which targets underwent simple harmonic motion, which is defined by a sinusoidal… Show more

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
3
2

Citation Types

0
8
0

Year Published

2015
2015
2021
2021

Publication Types

Select...
6
1

Relationship

1
6

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 10 publications
(8 citation statements)
references
References 34 publications
0
8
0
Order By: Relevance
“…It is also consistent with numerous imaging and electrophysiological studies that find an important role for premotor and supplementary motor areas in timing ( 59 63 ). In contrast, temporal cues may not play an active role during the adjustments that follow movement initiation when the brain has access to movement-related, state-dependent information ( 64 66 ).…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…It is also consistent with numerous imaging and electrophysiological studies that find an important role for premotor and supplementary motor areas in timing ( 59 63 ). In contrast, temporal cues may not play an active role during the adjustments that follow movement initiation when the brain has access to movement-related, state-dependent information ( 64 66 ).…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…the hand [ 43 45 ], the hitter tool [ 46 48 ] or the fingers in our case, depending on the task requirements. A possible explanation of our results is that moving faster allowed participants to reduce the effects of timing errors [ 12 , 13 , 46 , 49 , 50 ], and thus it is likely that contacting the ball at the time of highest hand closing speed increased success probability.…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 94%
“…A possible explanation for the observed unimodal distribution of PvClose for the touched trials ( Fig 9 , panel B), is that, in case of underestimated PvClose, participants were able to capture the ball also after it impacted on the palm and bounced off, hence gaining extra time for grasping motion execution. Similar to other interceptive tasks [ 12 , 13 , 47 ], ball capture in our experiment required to execute hand closing within a limited time window, which depended on ball velocity, size of the ball, the size of the hand defining a "catchable volume" ( Fig 11 ). For a successful grasping movement, a spatial error could be tolerated as long as the ball was within this catchable volume when the participants reached the final grip configuration.…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 99%
See 1 more Smart Citation
“…Time to contact (TTC), which is the time it takes for an object to reach an observer or a particular place, is an important factor in a variety of real-world situations, such as catching and hitting balls in games, driving vehicles, or passing through a busy street. The ability to estimate TTC for one object has been assessed in several studies (for example see [1][2][3][4][5][6][7][8][9][10][11][12][13][14]). The accuracy and precision of TTC estimation is related to the time perception ability, which is considered in several studies [6,11,[15][16][17][18][19][20][21][22][23][24].…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 99%