1979
DOI: 10.1016/b978-0-12-608602-7.50011-0
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Homonymy and Sound Change in the Child's Acquisition of Phonology

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
2
1
1

Citation Types

3
10
0

Year Published

1980
1980
2016
2016

Publication Types

Select...
8

Relationship

0
8

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 13 publications
(13 citation statements)
references
References 28 publications
3
10
0
Order By: Relevance
“…Specifically, the phonological representation differentiates target /r/ from target /w/ and the lexical representation differentiate target “wait” from target “rate.” This pattern is consistent with evidence of covert contrasts (e.g., Gierut & Dinnsen, 1986; Locke, 1979; Maxwell & Weismer, 1982; Tyler, Edwards, & Saxman, 1990; Weismer, Dinnsen, & Elbert, 1981). When a covert contrast is present, acoustic measures of production show that a child consistently produces a sound differently when it is being used as a substitute (e.g., [w] as a substitute for target /r/ in /reɪt/) than when it is being used target appropriately (e.g., [w] for target /w/ in /weɪt/).…”
Section: Introductionsupporting
confidence: 86%
“…Specifically, the phonological representation differentiates target /r/ from target /w/ and the lexical representation differentiate target “wait” from target “rate.” This pattern is consistent with evidence of covert contrasts (e.g., Gierut & Dinnsen, 1986; Locke, 1979; Maxwell & Weismer, 1982; Tyler, Edwards, & Saxman, 1990; Weismer, Dinnsen, & Elbert, 1981). When a covert contrast is present, acoustic measures of production show that a child consistently produces a sound differently when it is being used as a substitute (e.g., [w] as a substitute for target /r/ in /reɪt/) than when it is being used target appropriately (e.g., [w] for target /w/ in /weɪt/).…”
Section: Introductionsupporting
confidence: 86%
“…Up until now it has not been possible to estimate the full extent of covert contrasts in child speech, although some researchers predicted their occurrence to be widespread (e.g. Locke, 1979;Hewlett & Waters, 2004). Alongside perceptual studies, evidence from instrumental studies can provide essential complementary data about the articulatory and acoustic characteristics of covert contrasts in future research.…”
Section: Fiona E Gibbon and Alice Leementioning
confidence: 99%
“…Several researchers have viewed their occurrence as likely to be widespread. For example, Locke (1979) viewed phoneme omissions or substitutions as decreasing, but not eradicating, surface distinctiveness. He predicted that a large portion of children's contrasts that were judged by listeners as neutralisations would not turn out to be physiologically or acoustically neutralised.…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 97%
“…Although these phonemes are judged by listeners as homophonous, instrumental data can reveal that in reality the child produces the phonemes, in this case /d/ and /ɡ/, differently. The child's different productions of a phonological contrast that are heard by listeners as homophonous have come to be known as covert contrasts (Hewlett, 1988), although the same phenomenon has also been called sub-phonemic contrast and pseudohomonymy (Locke, 1979;Priestley, 1980). These contrasts are covert in the sense that adult listeners do not readily detect them and as a result they pass unrecorded in standard phonetic transcription.…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 96%