2016
DOI: 10.1016/j.jmp.2015.10.003
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

How Bayes factors change scientific practice

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
2
1
1

Citation Types

2
396
0
6

Year Published

2017
2017
2023
2023

Publication Types

Select...
9

Relationship

0
9

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 367 publications
(404 citation statements)
references
References 47 publications
(23 reference statements)
2
396
0
6
Order By: Relevance
“…A B of 0.33 or below indicates substantial evidence for H0 over H1 and a B between 3 and 0.33 indicates data-insensitivity for distinguishing between either hypothesis. Bayesian analysis is more appropriate for our data than null hypothesis significance testing (NHST) as: (i) we require our analysis to enable us to make statements on whether the null hypothesis can be accepted (whether infants treat colors equivalently), whereas NHST provides no measure of credibility in favor of the null and nonsignificant results do not enable a definite conclusion (39); (ii) we needed a statistical approach that guarantees sensitivity with a minimum number of participants (we have a between-subjects design with 16 conditions) and Bayes combined with an optional stopping rule (test until B is sensitive in either direction) enables this, as B retains its exact meaning as the evidence in favor of H1 over H0 with additional data collection (40); and (iii) Bayes factors should not be adjusted for multiple testing (we have 16 tests), as false-alarm rates are dealt with through information in the data with no reference to how many other tests are conducted (41). In addition to Bayes factors, we report associated P values from NHST, although these are for reference only because they are affected by the optional stopping rule and multiple testing.…”
Section: Resultsmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…A B of 0.33 or below indicates substantial evidence for H0 over H1 and a B between 3 and 0.33 indicates data-insensitivity for distinguishing between either hypothesis. Bayesian analysis is more appropriate for our data than null hypothesis significance testing (NHST) as: (i) we require our analysis to enable us to make statements on whether the null hypothesis can be accepted (whether infants treat colors equivalently), whereas NHST provides no measure of credibility in favor of the null and nonsignificant results do not enable a definite conclusion (39); (ii) we needed a statistical approach that guarantees sensitivity with a minimum number of participants (we have a between-subjects design with 16 conditions) and Bayes combined with an optional stopping rule (test until B is sensitive in either direction) enables this, as B retains its exact meaning as the evidence in favor of H1 over H0 with additional data collection (40); and (iii) Bayes factors should not be adjusted for multiple testing (we have 16 tests), as false-alarm rates are dealt with through information in the data with no reference to how many other tests are conducted (41). In addition to Bayes factors, we report associated P values from NHST, although these are for reference only because they are affected by the optional stopping rule and multiple testing.…”
Section: Resultsmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…whether p -values cross the level), and inferences are based on unobserved data (Cohen, 1994; Wagenmakers, 2007). Given those limitations, many have suggested alternative approaches such as model selection (Tibshirani, 1996; Yuan & Lin, 2006) or Bayesian statistics (Dienes, 2016; Wagenmakers, Morey, & Lee, 2016; Wagenmakers et al, 2017). Although each approach comes with pros and cons, here we extend on Bayesian hypothesis testing, a Bayesian alternative to NHST, and how it could be particularly useful in analysing conditioning data.…”
Section: P-values and Nhst For Threat Conditioning Datamentioning
confidence: 99%
“…For example, at the end of a threat acquisition phase, one could expect that the odds should be higher for the , indicating differences in CRs, than for the . However, in Bayesian hypothesis testing, those odds are often set to 1, indicating equal odds for both hypotheses (Dienes, 2016; Rouder, Speckman, Sun, Morey, & Iverson, 2009). In our example, those odds were also set to 1.…”
Section: Bayesian Hypothesis Testing For Threat Conditioning Datamentioning
confidence: 99%
“…In analyzing similar data, previous studies used null hypothesis significance testing. Although this approach may provide evidence for differences between groups (i.e., individuals with ASD differ from controls), it cannot distinguish between evidence that suggests no group differences and evidence that is inconclusive (i.e., does not provide enough evidence in support of either no group differences or group differences; Dienes, 2016). Therefore, we computed Bayes Factors (BFs) because they can distinguish between these two alternatives (Dienes, 2016;Wagenmakers, Morey, & Lee, 2016;Wiens & Nilsson, 2016).…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 99%