2021
DOI: 10.1016/j.anbehav.2021.01.015
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

How chimpanzees decide in the face of social and nonsocial uncertainty

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
1
1

Citation Types

4
12
0

Year Published

2022
2022
2024
2024

Publication Types

Select...
3
2

Relationship

1
4

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 6 publications
(16 citation statements)
references
References 67 publications
4
12
0
Order By: Relevance
“…It was only weakly correlated with the other risk domains (general risk: r = .10, 95% CI = [−.11, .31]) and negatively correlated with risk rank order ( r = –.25, 95% CI = [−.44, –.04]). These divergent findings mirror past results indicating that social risk taking involves an element of trust and thus is rooted in or codetermined by social preferences (Engelmann et al, 2015; Engelmann & Herrmann, 2016; Fehr, 2009; Haux et al, 2021). Next, we investigated the relationship between the observer report and the behavioral risk measure (Table S3 in the Supplemental Material).…”
Section: Resultssupporting
confidence: 83%
See 3 more Smart Citations
“…It was only weakly correlated with the other risk domains (general risk: r = .10, 95% CI = [−.11, .31]) and negatively correlated with risk rank order ( r = –.25, 95% CI = [−.44, –.04]). These divergent findings mirror past results indicating that social risk taking involves an element of trust and thus is rooted in or codetermined by social preferences (Engelmann et al, 2015; Engelmann & Herrmann, 2016; Fehr, 2009; Haux et al, 2021). Next, we investigated the relationship between the observer report and the behavioral risk measure (Table S3 in the Supplemental Material).…”
Section: Resultssupporting
confidence: 83%
“…In the commonly used setup, subjects make foraging decisions between a safe option (reliable outcome; e.g., one piece of food) and a risky option (variable option with outcome variance; e.g., 50% chance of getting two pieces of food or nothing). Depending on the methodology and the rewards involved, some studies have found that chimpanzees prove to be—like humans—risk averse, consistently preferring the safe over the risky option (Haux et al, 2021; Keupp et al, 2021); other studies suggest, however, that chimpanzees are risk seeking (Haun et al, 2011; Heilbronner et al, 2008; Rosati & Hare, 2011, 2012, 2013). Furthermore, there is some evidence of an increased sensitivity to social risks (see Calcutt et al, 2019; Haux et al, 2021; Rosati & Hare, 2012).…”
mentioning
confidence: 99%
See 2 more Smart Citations
“…However, consistent experimental findings show that human economic behaviours often appear irrational, i.e., not aimed at maximising utility [3]. Investigating the factors that lead to seemingly or real irrational choices shows that the way utility is assessed can highly modulate to generalise in primates, due to inter-species variability (for instance, bonobos appear more risk-averse than chimpanzees [24][25][26]) and between-study variance within a given primate species: Rhesus macaques appear risk-prone in [27][28][29] and risk-averse in [30], chimpanzees appear risk-prone in [31][32][33] and risk-averse in [34][35][36], and bonobos appear risk-prone in [19,32] and risk-averse in [24,25,31]. One major issue in this line of research is the diversity of the experimental designs that are used to test primates (see [37] for a review).…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 99%